No. 1 (2023)
Articles

Self-Focused but Lacking Self-Knowledge: The Relation Between Boredom and Self-Perception

Veerpal Bambrah
York University

Published 2023-02-12

Keywords

  • boredom,
  • self-perception,
  • self-directed attention,
  • self-knowledge

How to Cite

Bambrah, V., Moynihan, A. B., & Eastwood, J. D. (2023). Self-Focused but Lacking Self-Knowledge: The Relation Between Boredom and Self-Perception . Journal of Boredom Studies, (1). Retrieved from https://www.boredomsociety.com/jbs/index.php/journal/article/view/15

Abstract

Existing research suggests that people prone to boredom may have high self-directed attention (i.e., the tendency to focus on one’s inner experiences) but low self-knowledge (i.e., the tendency to possess knowledge of one’s inner experiences), which are two distinct aspects of self-perception. We empirically tested this proposal across multiple studies by examining the relationships between indices of boredom, self-directed attention, and self-knowledge. In Studies 1 and 2, we created a measure of state self-directed attention that possesses good psychometric properties, reliability, and convergent and construct validity. Additionally, we tested and confirmed the hypothesis that experimentally manipulating self-directed attention has no significant impact on boredom (Study 1), but that experimentally manipulating boredom causes a significant increase in self-directed attention (Study 2). In Study 3, we tested and confirmed the hypothesis that trait self-directed attention, trait self-knowledge, and trait boredom are correlated, but psychometrically distinct, dispositional constructs. We also tested and confirmed the hypothesis that trait self-directed attention and trait self-knowledge are uniquely associated with trait boredom (Study 3). Implications and future directions related to furthering our understanding of boredom and aspects of self-perception are discussed.

References

  1. Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2012). The Brain’s Default Network and Its Adaptive Role in Internal Mentation. The Neuroscientist, 18(3), 251–270. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858411403316
  2. Austin, P. C., and Steenberg, E. W. (2015). The Number of Subjects per Variable Required in Linear Regression Analyses. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 68(6), 627–636.
  3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.12.014
  4. Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., and Taylor, G. J. (1994). The Twenty-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—I. Item Selection and Cross-Validation of the Factor Structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 23–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1
  5. Bambrah, V., Wyman, A., and Eastwood, J. D. (2023). A Longitudinal Approach to Understanding Boredom during COVID-19: The Predictive Roles of Pandemic Trauma and Emotion Dysregulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 13.
  6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1050073
  7. Bargdill, R. W. (2000). The Study of Life Boredom. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 31(2), 188–219. https://doi.org/10.1163/15691620051090979
  8. Ben-Artzi, E. (2003). Factor Structure of the Private Self-Consciousness Scale: Role of Item Wording. Journal of Personality Assessment, 81(3), 256–264.
  9. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8103_08
  10. Brockmeyer, T., Zimmermann, J., Kulessa, D., Hautzinger, M., Bents, H., Friederich, H.-C., Herzog, W., and Backenstrass, M. (2015). Me, Myself, and I: Self-Referent Word Use as an Indicator of Self-Focused Attention in Relation to Depression and Anxiety. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01564
  11. Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit. Sociological Methods and Research, 21(2), 230–258. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124192021002005
  12. Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The Brain’s Default Network: Anatomy, Function, and Relevance to Disease. In A. Kingstone, and M. B. Miller (Eds.), The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience 2008 (pp. 1–38). Blackwell Publishing.
  13. Carlson, E. N. (2013). Overcoming Barriers to Self-Knowledge: Mindfulness as a Path to Seeing Yourself as You Really Are. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(2), 173–186. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612462584
  14. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond Boredom and Anxiety. Jossey-Bass Publishers.
  15. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. Harper and Row.
  16. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). Flow: The Classic Work on How to Achieve Happiness. Rider.
  17. Danckert, J., and Isacescu, J. (2017). The Bored Brain: Insular Cortex and the Default Mode Network. PsyArXiv Preprints.
  18. Danckert, J., and Merrifield, C. (2018). Boredom, Sustained Attention and the Default Mode Network. Experimental Brain Research, 236(9), 2507–2518. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4617-5
  19. Darwin, C. (1965). The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals. The University of Chicago Press.
  20. Duval, T. S., and Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A Theory of Objective Self-Awareness. Academic Press.
  21. Eastwood, J. D., Cavaliere, C., Fahlman, S. A., and Eastwood, A. E. (2007). A Desire for Desires: Boredom and Its Relation to Alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 1035–1045. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.027
  22. Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., and Smilek, D. (2012). The Unengaged Mind: Defining Boredom in Terms of Attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 482–495. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612456044
  23. Eastwood, J. D., and Gorelik, D. (2019). Boredom Is a Feeling of Thinking and a Double-Edged Sword. In J. Ros Velasco (Ed.), Boredom Is in Your Mind: A Shared Psychological-Philosophical Approach (pp. 55–70). Springer.
  24. Elpidorou, A. (2018). The Good of Boredom. Philosophical Psychology, 31(3), 323–351. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2017.1346240
  25. Fahlman, S. A., Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., and Eastwood, J. D. (2013). Development and Validation of the Multidimensional State Boredom Scale. Assessment, 20(1), 68–85. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111421303
  26. Farmer, R., and Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom Proneness: The Development and Correlates of a New Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50(1), 4–17.
  27. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5001_2
  28. Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., and Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and Private Self-Consciousness: Assessment and Theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43(4), 522–527. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076760
  29. Flora, D. B., and Bell, S. (2021, June). Effect Sizes in Structural Equal Models. 82nd Canadian Psychological Association Annual National Convention, Toronto, Canada.
  30. Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Christoff, K. (2015). The Wandering Brain: Meta-Analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Mind-Wandering and Related Spontaneous Thought Processes. NeuroImage, 111, 611–621.
  31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.039
  32. Gana, K., Deletang, B., and Metais, L. (2000). Is Boredom Proneness Associated with Introspectiveness? Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 28(5), 499–504. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2000.28.5.499
  33. Gibbons, F. X. (1983). Self-Attention and Self-Report: The “Veridicality” Hypothesis. Journal of Personality, 51(3), 517–542. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1983.tb00343.x
  34. Goldberg, Y. K., Eastwood, J. D., LaGuardia, J., and Danckert, J. (2011). Boredom: An Emotional Experience Distinct from Apathy, Anhedonia, or Depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30(6), 647–666. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2011.30.6.647
  35. Goor, D. J., Schur, M. (Writers), Lord, P., and Miller, C. (Directors) (2013). Pilot [Television series episode.] In D. J. Goor, C. Miller, D. Miner, P. Lord, and M. Schur (Executive producers), Brooklyn Nine-Nine. Fox Broadcasting.
  36. Gorelik, D., and Eastwood, J. D. (Under review). Development of the Trait Boredom Scale.
  37. Govern, J. M., and Marsch, L. A. (2001). Development and Validation of the Situational Self-Awareness Scale. Consciousness and Cognition, 10(3), 366–378.
  38. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.2001.0506
  39. Gusnard, D. A., and Raichle, M. E. (2001). Searching for a Baseline: Functional Imaging and the Resting Human Brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(10), 685–694.
  40. https://doi.org/10.1038/35094500
  41. Hansell, S., Mechanic, D., and Brondolo, E. (1986). Introspectiveness and Adolescent Development. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 15(2), 115–132.
  42. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02141733
  43. Hardie-Bick, J., and Scott, S. (2017). Waiting in the Wings: Critical Self-Reflection and the Disruption of Flow. Self & Society, 47(1). 59–68. http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/72758/
  44. Harrington, R., and Loffredo, D. A. (2010). Insight, Rumination, and Self-Reflection as Predictors of Well-Being. The Journal of Psychology, 145(1), 39–57.
  45. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2010.528072
  46. Harris, M. B. (2000). Correlates and Characteristics of Boredom Proneness and Boredom. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(3), 576–598.
  47. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02497.x
  48. Havermans, R. C., Vancleef, L., Kalamatianos, A., and Nederkoorn, C. (2015). Eating and Inflicting Pain out of Boredom. Appetite, 85, 52–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.11.007
  49. Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., and Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential Avoidance and Behavioral Disorders: A Functional Dimensional Approach to Diagnosis and Treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(6), 1152–1168.
  50. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.64.6.1152
  51. Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria Versus New Alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
  52. Hunter, J. A., Dyer, K. J., Cribbie, R. A., and Eastwood, J. D. (2015). Exploring the Utility of the Multidimensional State Boredom Scale. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 32(3), 241–250. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000251
  53. Kılıç, A., Tilburg, W. A. P., and Igou, E. R. (2019). Risk‐Taking Increases under Boredom. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 33(3), 257–269. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2160
  54. Lai, K., and Green, S. B. (2016). The Problem with Having Two Watches: Assessment of Fit When RMSEA and CFI Disagree. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 51(2–3), 220–239. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2015.1134306
  55. Lambie, J. A., and Marcel, A. J. (2002). Consciousness and the Varieties of Emotion Experience: A Theoretical Framework. Psychological Review, 109(2), 219–259.
  56. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.109.2.219
  57. LePera, N. (2011). Relationships Between Boredom Proneness, Mindfulness, Anxiety, Depression, and Substance Use. PsycEXTRA Dataset. https://doi.org/10.1037/e741452011-003
  58. MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., and Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power Analysis and Determination of Sample Size for Covariance Structure Modeling. Psychological Methods, 1(2), 130–149. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.1.2.130
  59. Markey, A., Chin, A., VanEpps, E. M., and Loewenstein, G. (2014). Across the Bored: Identifying a Reliable Boredom Induction. PsycEXTRA Dataset.
  60. https://doi.org/10.1037/e642622013-084
  61. Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., and Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought. Science, 315(5810), 393–395. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1131295
  62. McDonald, R. P., and Ho, M.-H. R. (2002). Principles and Practice in Reporting Structural Equation Analyses. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 64–82. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.64
  63. Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., Fahlman, S. A., and Eastwood, J. D. (2011). The Measurement of Boredom: Differences Between Existing Self-Report Scales. Assessment, 20(5), 585–596. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111408229
  64. Mor, N., Doane, L. D., Adam, E. K., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R. E., Griffith, J. W., Craske, M. G., Waters, A., and Nazarian, M. (2010). Within-Person Variations in Self-Focused Attention and Negative Affect in Depression and Anxiety: A Diary Study. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 48–62. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802499715
  65. Mor, N., and Winquist, J. (2002). Self-Focused Attention and Negative Affect: A Meta-Analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 638–662. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.638
  66. Morin, A. (2017). Toward a Glossary of Self-Related Terms. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00280
  67. Moynihan, A. B., Igou, E. R., and Van Tilburg, W. A. (2017). Boredom Increases Impulsiveness. Social Psychology, 48(5), 293–309. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000317
  68. Moynihan, A. B., Igou, E. R., and Van Tilburg, W. A. (2021). Existential Escape of the Bored: A Review of Meaning-Regulation Processes under Boredom. European Review of Social Psychology, 32(1), 161–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2020.1829347
  69. Moynihan, A. B., Van Tilburg, W. A., Igou, E. R., Wisman, A., Donnelly, A. E., and Mulcaire, J. B. (2015). Eaten up by Boredom: Consuming Food to Escape Awareness of the Bored Self. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00369
  70. Mugon, J., Struk, A., and Danckert, J. (2018). A Failure to Launch: Regulatory Modes and Boredom Proneness. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01126
  71. Panayiotou, G., Leonidou, C., Constantinou, E., Hart, J., Rinehart, K. L., Sy, J. T., and Björgvinsson, T. (2015). Do Alexithymic Individuals Avoid Their Feelings? Experiential Avoidance Mediates the Association Between Alexithymia, Psychosomatic, and Depressive Symptoms in a Community and a Clinical Sample. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 56, 206–216.
  72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.09.006
  73. Seib, H. M., and Vodanovich, S. J. (1998). Cognitive Correlates of Boredom Proneness: The Role of Private Self-Consciousness and Absorption. The Journal of Psychology, 132(6), 642–652. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223989809599295
  74. Silvia, P. J., and Duval, T. S. (2001). Objective Self-Awareness Theory: Recent Progress and Enduring Problems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(3), 230–241. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0503_4
  75. Silvia, P. J., and Eichstaedt, J. (2004). A Self-Novelty Manipulation of Self-Focused Attention for Internet and Laboratory Experiments. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(2), 325–330. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03195578
  76. Struk, A. A., Carriere, J. S., Cheyne, J. A., and Danckert, J. (2015a). A Short Boredom Proneness Scale: Development and Psychometric Properties. Assessment, 24(3), 346–359.
  77. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191115609996
  78. Struk, A. A., Scholer, A. A., and Danckert, J. (2015b). A Self-Regulatory Approach to Understanding Boredom Proneness. Cognition and Emotion, 30(8), 1388–1401.
  79. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1064363
  80. Swinkels, A., and Giuliano, T. A. (1995). The Measurement and Conceptualization of Mood Awareness: Monitoring and Labeling One’s Mood States. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(9), 934–949. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295219008
  81. Tam, K. Y., Van Tilburg, W. A., Chan, C. S., Igou, E. R., and Lau, H. (2021). Attention Drifting in and out: The Boredom Feedback Model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 25(3), 251–272. https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683211010297
  82. Tolstoy, L. N. (1899). Anna Karenina. Thomas Y. Crowell Co.
  83. Trapnell, P. D., and Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private Self-Consciousness and the Five-Factor Model of Personality: Distinguishing Rumination from Reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(2), 284–304. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.284
  84. Ulrich, M., Keller, J., Hoenig, K., Waller, C., and Grön, G. (2014). Neural Correlates of Experimentally Induced Flow Experiences. NeuroImage, 86, 194–202.
  85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.019
  86. Venta, A., Hart, J., and Sharp, C. (2012). The Relation Between Experiential Avoidance, Alexithymia and Emotion Regulation in Inpatient Adolescents. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18(3), 398–410. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104512455815
  87. Vodanovich, S. J., and Watt, J. D. (2016). Self-Report Measures of Boredom: An Updated Review of the Literature. The Journal of Psychology, 150(2), 196–228.
  88. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2015.1074531
  89. von Gemmingen, M. J., Sullivan, B. F., and Pomerantz, A. M. (2003). Investigating the Relationships Between Boredom Proneness, Paranoia, and Self-Consciousness. Personality and Individual Differences, 34(6), 907–919. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0191-8869(01)00219-7
  90. Wangh, M. (1975). Boredom in Psychoanalytic Perspective. Sociological Research, 42(3), 538–550. https://www.jstor.org/stable/41582848
  91. Wilson, T. D. (2009). Know Thyself. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(4), 384–389. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01143.x
  92. Wilson Van Voorhis, C. R., and Morgan, B. L. (2007). Understanding Power and Rules of Thumb for Determining Sample Sizes. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 3(2), 43–50. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.03.2.p043
  93. Zinck, A. (2008). Self-Referential Emotions. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 496–505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.014