No. 2 (2024)
Articles

Role of Context in Daily Boredom Experiences During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Daily Diary Study

Sumana Sri
Claremont Graduate University

Published 2024-10-14

Keywords

  • COVID-19,
  • boredom,
  • daily meaning in life,
  • daily diary,
  • social contexts

How to Cite

Sri, S. (2024). Role of Context in Daily Boredom Experiences During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Daily Diary Study. Journal of Boredom Studies, (2). Retrieved from https://www.boredomsociety.com/jbs/index.php/journal/article/view/28

Abstract

Boredom is typically an experience from which most people want to escape. However, what happens when external constraints are placed on people’s lives such as living through a pandemic and having to follow lockdown protocols? How does this affect their boredom experiences? In this 7-day daily diary study with 289 adults from the United States, we empirically tested how the changes in people’s daily lives brought on by the COVID-19 pandemic, specifically, the amount of time they spent at home and the amount of time they spent alone, influenced the level of boredom they experienced on a given day. We also tested how the changes in people’s daily meaning in life, which had also been impacted by the pandemic, influenced people’s daily boredom experiences. Results from multilevel modeling showed that on days when people spent more time alone and experienced lower daily meaning in life, they experienced higher levels of boredom. However, the amount of time spent at home did not predict daily boredom. This study contributes to boredom literature by highlighting the impact that contextual factors have on people’s boredom experiences. Typically, psychological research on boredom has focused on the characteristics of a specific activity that people are engaged in, or by their dispositional propensity to feeling bored. Understanding the role of people’s contexts can help us devise nuanced and effective boredom coping interventions.

References

  1. Acee, T. W., Kim, H., Kim, H. J., Kim, J. I., Chu, H. N. R., Kim, M., and Boredom Research Group. (2010). Academic Boredom in under and over Challenging Situations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 17–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2009.08.002
  2. Ballew, M., Bergquist, P., Goldberg, M., Gustafson, A., Kotcher, J., Marlon, J., Roess, A., Rosenthal, S., Maibach, E., and Leiserowitz, A. (2020). Americans’ Risk Perceptions and Emotional Responses to COVID-19, April 2020. Yale University and George Mason University. Yale Program on Climate Change Communication.
  3. Baratta, P. L., and Spence, J. R. (2015). A Riddle, Wrapped in a Mystery, Inside an Enigma… or Just Multidimensional? Testing the Multidimensional Structure of Boredom. In P. L. Baratta and J. R. Spence (Eds.), New Ways of Studying Emotions in Organizations (vol. 11, pp. 139–172). Emerald Group Publishing Limited.
  4. Barbalet, J. M. (1999). Boredom and Social Meaning. The British Journal of Sociology, 50(4), 631–646. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-4446.1999.00631.x
  5. Bargdill, R. (2000). The Study of Life Boredom. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 31(2), 188–219. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1163/15691620051090979
  6. Behan, C. (2014). Learning to Escape: Prison Education, Rehabilitation and the Potential for Transformation. Journal of Prison Education and Reentry, 1(1), 20–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.15845/jper.v1i1.594
  7. Bench, S. W., and Lench, H. C. (2019). Boredom as a Seeking State: Boredom Prompts the Pursuit of Novel (even Negative) Experiences. Emotion, 19(2), 242–254. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000433
  8. Bengtsson, T. T. (2012). Boredom and Action: Experiences from Youth Confinement. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 41(5), 526–553. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891241612449356
  9. Brent, D. A., and Birmaher, B. (2002). Adolescent Depression. New England Journal of Medicine, 347(9), 667–671. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra2033475
  10. Britt, T. W., Sytine, A., Brady, A., Wilkes, R., Pittman, R., Jennings, K., and Goguen, K. (2017). Enhancing the Meaningfulness of Work for Astronauts on Long Duration Space Exploration Missions. Aerospace Medicine and Human Performance, 88(8), 779–783. https://doi.org/10.3357/amhp.4875.2017
  11. Chin, A., Markey, A., Bhargava, S., Kassam, K. S., and Loewenstein, G. (2017). Bored in the USA: Experience Sampling and Boredom in Everyday Life. Emotion, 17(2), 359–368. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000232
  12. Cleary, M., Sayers, J., Lopez, V., and Hungerford, C. (2016). Boredom in the Workplace: Reasons, Impact, and Solutions. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 37(2), 83–89. https://doi.org/10.3109/01612840.2015.1084554
  13. Cohen-Mansfield, J., Hazan, H., Lerman, Y., and Shalom, V. (2016). Correlates and Predictors of Loneliness in Older Adults: A Review of Quantitative Results Informed by Qualitative Insights. International Psychogeriatrics, 28(4), 557–576. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1041610215001532
  14. Collins, D. L. (2003). Psychological Issues Relevant to Astronaut Selection for Long-Duration Space Flight: A Review of the Literature. Journal of Human Performance in Extreme Environments, 7(1), 43–67. https://doi.org/10.7771/2327-2937.1021
  15. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond Boredom and Anxiety. Jossey-Bass.
  16. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. Harper Perennial.
  17. Darden, D., and Marks, A. (1999). Boredom: A Socially Devaluated Emotion. Sociological Spectrum, 19(1), 13–37.
  18. Delpino, F. M., da Silva, C. N., Jerônimo, J. S., Mulling, E. S., da Cunha, L. L., Weymar, M. K., Alt, R., Caputo, E. L., and Feter, N. (2022). Prevalence of Anxiety during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of over 2 Million People. Journal of Affective Disorders, 318(1), 272–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.09.003
  19. Dora, J., Van Hooff, M., Geurts, S., Kompier, M., and Bijleveld, E. (2021). Fatigue, Boredom and Objectively Measured Smartphone Use at Work. Royal Society Open Science, 8(7), 201915. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.201915
  20. Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., and Smilek, D. (2012). The Unengaged Mind: Defining Boredom in Terms of Attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 482–495. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612456044
  21. Eaton, L. G., and Funder, D. C. (2001). Emotional Experience in Daily Life: Valence, Variability, and Rate of Change. Emotion, 1(4), 413–421. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.1.4.413
  22. Ekwonye, A. U., Ezumah, B. A., and Nwosisi, N. (2021). Meaning in Life and Impact of COVID-19 Pandemic on African Immigrants in the United States. Wellbeing, Space and Society, 2(1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wss.2021.100033
  23. Elpidorou, A. (2018). The Good of Boredom. Philosophical Psychology, 31(3), 323–351. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2017.1346240
  24. Elpidorou, A. (2022). Boredom and Cognitive Engagement: A Functional Theory of Boredom. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 13(2), 1–30. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00599-6
  25. Fahlman, S. A., Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., and Eastwood, J. D. (2013). Development and Validation of the Multidimensional State Boredom Scale. Assessment, 20(1), 68–85. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111421303
  26. Farmer, R., and Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom Proneness: The Development and Correlates of a New Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50(1), 4–17. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5001_2
  27. Finkielsztein, M. (2023). The Significance of Boredom: A Literature Review. Journal of Boredom Studies, 1(1), 1–33. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7144313
  28. Garner, J. (2020). Experiencing Time in Prison: The Influence of Books, Libraries and Reading. Journal of Documentation, 76(5), 1033–1050. https://doi.org/10.1108/JD-07-2019-0128
  29. Gatti, M., Palumbo, R., Di Domenico, A., and Mammarella, N. (2022). Affective Health and Countermeasures in Long-duration Space Exploration. Heliyon, 8(5), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2022.e09414
  30. Goldberg, Y. K., Eastwood, J. D., LaGuardia, J., and Danckert, J. (2011). Boredom: An Emotional Experience Distinct from Apathy, Anhedonia, or Depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30(6), 647–666. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1521/jscp.2011.30.6.647
  31. Hamilton, J. A. (1983). Development of Interest and Enjoyment in Adolescence. Part II. Boredom and Psychopathology. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 12(5), 363–372. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02088720
  32. Harris, M. B. (2000). Correlates and Characteristics of Boredom Proneness and Boredom. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(3), 576–598. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02497.x
  33. Havermans, R. C., Vancleef, L., Kalamatianos, A., and Nederkoorn, C. (2015). Eating and Inflicting Pain out of Boredom. Appetite, 85(1), 52–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.11.007
  34. Hill, P. L., Klaiber, P., Burrow, A. L., DeLongis, A., and Sin, N. L. (2022). Purposefulness and Daily Life in a Pandemic: Predicting Daily Affect and Physical Symptoms during the First Weeks of the COVID-19 Response. Psychology & Health, 37(8), 985–1001. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2021.1914838
  35. Ipsen, C., Van Veldhoven, M., Kirchner, K., and Hansen, J. P. (2021). Six Key Advantages and Disadvantages of Working from Home in Europe during COVID-19. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(4), 1826. https://doi.org/10.3390%2Fijerph18041826
  36. Irawan, A. W., Dwisona, D., and Lestari, M. (2020). Psychological Impacts of Students on Online Learning during the Pandemic COVID-19. Journal Bimbingan dan Konseling, 7(1), 53–60. https://doi.org/10.4236/ce.2021.126097
  37. Iso-Ahola, S. E., and Crowley, E. D. (1991). Adolescent Substance Abuse and Leisure Boredom. Journal of Leisure Research, 23(3), 260–271.
  38. Jackson, A., Anderson, A., Weybright, E., and Lanigan, J. (2021). Differing Experiences of Boredom during the Pandemic and Associations with Dietary Behaviors. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 53(8), 706–711. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2021.04.005
  39. Kashdan, T. B., and Steger, M. F. (2007). Curiosity and Pathways to Well-being and Meaning in Life: Traits, States, and Everyday Behaviors. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 159–173. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9068-7
  40. Khanijahani, A., Iezadi, S., Gholipour, K., Azami-Aghdash, S., and Naghibi, D. (2021). A Systematic Review of Racial/Ethnic and Socioeconomic Disparities in COVID-19. International Journal for Equity in Health, 20(1), 1–30. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12939-021-01582-4
  41. Kuhn, R. (1976). The Demon of Noontide. Ennui in Western Literature. Princeton University Press.
  42. Kılıç, A., Van Tilburg, W. A., and Igou, E. R. (2020). Risk‐taking Increases under Boredom. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 33(3), 257–269. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2160
  43. Krok, D. (2018). When Is Meaning in Life Most Beneficial to Young People? Styles of Meaning in Life and Well-being among Late Adolescents. Journal of Adult Development, 25(2), 96–106. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-017-9280
  44. Laghi, F., Pompili, S., Baumgartner, E., and Baiocco, R. (2015). The Role of Sensation Seeking and Motivations for Eating in Female and Male Adolescents Who Binge Eat. Eating Behaviors, 17, 119–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.01.011
  45. Larson, R. W. (1990). The Solitary Side of Life: An Examination of the Time People Spend Alone from Childhood to Old Age. Developmental Review, 10(2), 155–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(90)90008-R
  46. Larson, R. W., and Richards, M. H. (1991). Boredom in the Middle School Years: Blaming Schools Versus Blaming Students. American Journal of Education, 99(4), 418–443. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1086/443992
  47. Larson, R. W., Moneta, G., Richards, M. H., and Wilson, S. (2002). Continuity, Stability, and Change in Daily Emotional Experience across Adolescence. Child Development, 73(4), 1151–1165. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00464
  48. Laws, J. M., Bruce-Martin, C., Caplan, N., Meroni, R., and Winnard, A. (2022). Exercise Countermeasure Preferences of Three Male Astronauts: A Preliminary Qualitative Study. Acta Astronautica, 201, 224–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actaastro.2022.09.012
  49. Layland, E. K., Ram, N., Caldwell, L. L., Smith, E. A., and Wegner, L. (2021). Leisure Boredom, Timing of Sexual Debut, and Cooccurring Behaviors among South African Adolescents. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 50(6), 2383–2394. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-021-02014-8
  50. Liang, Z., Zhao, Q., Zhou, Z., Yu, Q., Li, S., and Chen, S. (2020). The Effect of “Novelty Input” and “Novelty Output” on Boredom during Home Quarantine in the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Moderating Effects of Trait Creativity. Frontiers in Psychology, 11(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.601548
  51. Long, C. R., Seburn, M., Averill, J. R., and More, T. A. (2003). Solitude Experiences: Varieties, Settings, and Individual Differences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(5), 578–583. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203029005003
  52. Machell, K. A., Kashdan, T. B., Short, J. L., and Nezlek, J. B. (2015). Relationships between Meaning in Life, Social and Achievement Events, and Positive and Negative Affect in Daily Life. Journal of Personality, 83(3), 287–298. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12103
  53. Martin, M., Sadlo, G., and Stew, G. (2006). The Phenomenon of Boredom. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3, 193–211.
  54. Martz, M. E., Schulenberg, J. E., Patrick, M. E., and Kloska, D. D. (2018). "I Am So Bored!": Prevalence Rates and Sociodemographic and Contextual Correlates of High Boredom among American Adolescents. Youth & Society, 50(5), 688–710. https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118X15626624
  55. McKenna-Plumley, P. E., Graham-Wisener, L., Berry, E., and Groarke, J. M. (2021). Connection, Constraint, and Coping: A Qualitative Study of Experiences of Loneliness during the COVID-19 Lockdown in the UK. PLoS One, 16(10), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258344
  56. McRobbie, L. R. (2012, November). The History of Boredom. Smithsonian. http://www.smithsonianmag.com/science-nature/the-history-of-boredom-138176427/
  57. Merrifield, C., and Danckert, J. (2014). Characterizing the Psychophysiological Signature of Boredom. Experimental Brain Research, 232(2), 481–91. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3755-2
  58. Miller, J. A., Caldwell, L. L., Weybright, E. H., Smith, E. A., Vergnani, T., and Wegner, L. (2014). Was Bob Seger Right? Relation between Boredom in Leisure and [Risky] Sex. Leisure Sciences, 36(1), 52–67. https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01490400.2014.860789
  59. Mikulas, W. L., and Vodanovich, S. J. (1993). The Essence of Boredom. The Psychological Record, 43(1), 3–12.
  60. Misztal, B. (2016). The Ambiguity of Everyday Experience: Between Normality and Boredom. Qualitative Sociology Review, 12(4), 100–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.18778/1733-8077.12.4.06
  61. Musharbash, Y. (2007). Boredom, Time, and Modernity: An Example from Aboriginal Australia. American Anthropologist, 109(2), 307–317. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1525/aa.2007.109.2.307
  62. Nederkoorn, C., Vancleef, L., Wilkenhöner, A., Claes, L., and Havermans, R. C. (2016). Self-inflicted Pain out of Boredom. Psychiatry Research, 237, 127–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.063
  63. Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., and Hall, N. C. (2011). Coping with Boredom in School: An Experience Sampling Perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36(1), 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.003
  64. Newman, D. B., Nezlek, J. B., and Thrash, T. M. (2018). The Dynamics of Searching for Meaning and Presence of Meaning in Daily Life. Journal of Personality, 86(3), 368–379. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12321
  65. Nezlek, J. B. (2008). An Introduction to Multilevel Modeling for Social and Personality Psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(2), 842–860. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00059.x
  66. Ohlmeier, S., Finkielsztein, M., and Pfaff, H. (2020). Why We Are Bored: Towards a Sociological Approach to Boredom. Sociological Spectrum, 40(3), 208–225. https://doi.org/10.1080/02732173.2020.1753134
  67. Patel, J. A., Nielsen, F. B. H., Badiani, A. A., Assi, S., Unadkat, V. A., Patel, B., and Wardle, H. (2020). Poverty, Inequality and COVID-19: The Forgotten Vulnerable. Public Health, 183(1), 110–111. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.puhe.2020.05.006
  68. Patterson, I., and Pegg, S. (1999). Nothing to Do: The Relationship between Leisure Boredom and Alcohol and Drug Addiction: Is There a Link to Youth Suicide in Rural Australia? Youth Studies Australia, 18(2), 24–29.
  69. Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., and Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in Achievement Settings: Exploring Control-value Antecedents and Performance Outcomes of a Neglected Emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 531–549. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0019243
  70. Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., Goetz, T., and Perry, R. P. (2014). Boredom and Academic Achievement: Testing a Model of Reciprocal Causation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(3), 696–710. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0036006
  71. Petry, N. M. (2001). Substance Abuse, Pathological Gambling, and Impulsiveness. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 63, 29–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0376-8716(00)00188-5
  72. Raffaelli, Q., Mills, C., and Christoff, K. (2018). The Knowns and Unknowns of Boredom: A Review of the Literature. Experimental Brain Research, 236(9), 2451–2462. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-4922-7
  73. Rocheleau, A. M. (2013). An Empirical Exploration of the “Pains of Imprisonment” and the Level of Prison Misconduct and Violence. Criminal Justice Review, 38(3), 354–374. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734016813494764
  74. Sharp, E. H., Coffman, D. L., Caldwell, L. L., Smith, E. A., Wegner, L., Vergnani, T., and Mathews, C. (2011). Predicting Substance Use Behavior among South African Adolescents: The Role of Leisure Experiences across Time. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35(4), 343–351. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0165025411404494
  75. Shaw, S. M., Caldwell, L. L., and Kleiber, D. A. (1996). Boredom, Stress and Social Control in the Daily Activities of Adolescents. Journal of Leisure Research, 28(4), 274–292. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222216.1996.11949776
  76. Spaeth, M., Weichold, K., and Silbereisen, R. K. (2015). The Development of Leisure Boredom in Early Adolescence: Predictors and Longitudinal Associations with Delinquency and Depression. Developmental Psychology, 51(10), 1380–1394. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039480
  77. Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., Sullivan, B. A., and Lorentz, D. (2008). Understanding the Search for Meaning in Life: Personality, Cognitive Style, and the Dynamic between Seeking and Experiencing Meaning. Journal of Personality, 76(2), 199–228. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00484.x
  78. Tao, Y., Niu, H., Hou, W., Zhang, L., and Ying, R. (2022). Hopelessness during and after the COVID‐19 Pandemic Lockdown among Chinese College Students: A Longitudinal Network Analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 79(3), 748–761. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.23439
  79. Trzebiński, J., Cabański, M., and Czarnecka, J. Z. (2020). Reaction to the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Influence of Meaning in Life, Life Satisfaction, and Assumptions on World Orderliness and Positivity. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 25(6–7), 544–557. https://doi.org/10.1080/15325024.2020.1765098
  80. Unni, Z., and Weinstein, E. (2021). Shelter in Place, Connect Online: Trending TikTok Content during the Early Days of the US Covid-19 Pandemic. Journal of Adolescent Health, 68(5), 863–868. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2021.02.012
  81. Van Tilburg, W. A. P., and Igou, E. R. (2012). On Boredom: Lack of Challenge and Meaning as Distinct Boredom Experiences. Motivation and Emotion, 36(2), 181–194. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1007/s11031-011-9234-9
  82. Van Tilburg, W. A. P., Pekrun, R., and Igou, E. R. (2022). Consumed by Boredom: Food Choice Motivation and Weight Changes during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Behavioral Sciences, 12(10), 366. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100366
  83. Vodanovich, S. J., and Kass, S. J. (1990). A Factor Analytic Study of the Boredom Proneness Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55(1–2), 115–123. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.1990.9674051
  84. Vodanovich, S. J., and Watt, J. D. (2016). Self-report Measures of Boredom: An Updated Review of the Literature. The Journal of Psychology, 150(2), 196–228. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2015.1074531
  85. Weingarten, N., Römer, S., Klein, S., Marszalek, K., van der Meer, I., Nijensteen, M., and Stranc, S. (2016). The Effect of Frustration and Boredom on Self-harming Behaviour. Maastricht Student Journal of Psychology and Neuroscience, 5(1), 66–82.
  86. Westgate, E. C. (2020). Why Boredom Is Interesting. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(1), 33–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419884309
  87. Westgate, E. C., and Wilson, T. D. (2018). Boring Thoughts and Bored Minds: The MAC Model of Boredom and Cognitive Engagement. Psychological Review, 125(5), 689–713. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000097
  88. Wilson, T. D., Reinhard, D. A., Westgate, E. C., Gilbert, D. T., Ellerbeck, N., Hahn, C., and Shaked, A. (2014). Just Think: The Challenges of the Disengaged Mind. Science, 345, 75–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1250830
  89. Yusoufzai, M. K., Vancleef, L., Lobbestael, J., and Schulte, H. M. (2022). Painfully Bored: The Role of Negative Urgency and History of Non-suicidal Self-injury in Self-administering Painful Stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 46(6), 689–701. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-022-09970-1
  90. Zhang, J. (2021). People’s Responses to the COVID-19 Pandemic during Its Early Stages and Factors Affecting those Responses. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 8(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00720-1