Published 2024-11-22
Keywords
- boredom proneness,
- attention,
- cognitive flexibility,
- memory,
- emotion regulation
How to Cite
Copyright (c) 2024 Natasha Vogel, Mark Fenske
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Abstract
The association between boredom proneness and elevated rates of problematic substance abuse, gambling, and smartphone use has been taken as evidence that difficulties with emotion regulation can lead to maladaptive attempts to cope with negative affect. There is minimal research on how individual differences in emotion regulation may be linked to boredom proneness. We therefore sought to identify specific aspects of emotion regulation that may be helpful for predicting boredom proneness. We hypothesized that boredom proneness may be associated with aspects of emotion regulation that are often unproductive (e.g., suppression and rumination) or that rely on effective executive functions (e.g., attention, working memory, cognitive flexibility). Undergraduate students (N = 219) completed a battery of self-report scales regarding their boredom proneness, emotion-regulation abilities, and cognitive abilities, including attention, memory and cognitive flexibility. Results indicated that difficulties in emotion regulation predicted boredom proneness and was mediated by attentional difficulties and lower levels of cognitive flexibility, but not memory failures. Individual differences in emotion-suppression and rumination were predictive of boredom proneness, but the use of distraction was not. Our results underscore the importance of specific cognitive-affective mechanisms of emotion regulation to better understand boredom proneness and its long-term consequences.
References
- Adler, L. A., Spencer, T., Faraone, S. V., Kessler, R. C., Howes, M. J., Biederman, J., and Secnik, K. (2006). Validity of Pilot Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) to Rate Adult ADHD symptoms. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry: Official Journal of the American Academy of Clinical Psychiatrists, 18(3), 145–148. https://doi.org/10.1080/10401230600801077
- Aldao, A. (2013). The Future of Emotion Regulation Research: Capturing Context. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(2), 155–172. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612459518
- Aldao, A., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Specificity of Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies: A Transdiagnostic Examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(10), 974–983.
- Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion Regulation Strategies across Psychopathology: A Meta-Analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. https://doi.org.10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004
- Bambrah, V., Wyman, A., and Eastwood, J. D. (2023). A Longitudinal Approach to Understanding Boredom during Pandemics: The Predictive Roles of Trauma and Emotion Dysregulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1050073
- Bench, S. W., and Lench, H. C. (2019). Boredom as a Seeking State: Boredom Prompts the Pursuit of Novel (Even Negative) Experiences. Emotion, 19(2), 242–254. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000433
- Bieleke, M., Barton, L., and Wolff, W. (2021). Trajectories of Boredom in Self-Control Demanding Tasks. Cognition and Emotion, 35(5), 1018–1028. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2021.1901656
- Bjureberg, J., Ljótsson, B., Tull, M. T., Hedman, E., Sahlin, H., Lundh, L.-G., Bjärehed, J., DiLillo, D., Messman-Moore, T., Gumpert, C. H., and Gratz, K. L. (2016). Development and Validation of a Brief Version of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale: The DERS-16. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 38(2), 284–296. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-015-9514-x
- Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The Benefits of Being Present: Mindfulness and Its Role in Psychological Well-Being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
- Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A., and Smilek, D. (2008). Everyday Attention Lapses and Memory Failures: The Affective Consequences of Mindlessness. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 835–847. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.008
- Chin, A., Markey, A., Bhargava, S., Kassam, K. S., and Loewenstein, G. (2017). Bored in the USA: Experience Sampling and Boredom in Everyday Life. Emotion, 17(2), 359–368. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000232
- Crockett, A. C., Myhre, S. K., and Rokke, P. D. (2015). Boredom Proneness and Emotion Regulation Predict Emotional Eating. Journal of Health Psychology, 20(5), 670–680. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105315573439
- De France, K., and Hollenstein, T. (2017). Assessing Emotion Regulation Repertoires: The Regulation of Emotion Systems Survey. Personality and Individual Differences, 119, 204–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.07.018
- Derryberry, D., and Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-Related Attentional Biases and Their Regulation by Attentional Control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 225–236. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.225
- Eastwood, J. D., Cavaliere, C., Fahlman, S. A., and Eastwood, A. E. (2007). A Desire for Desires: Boredom and Its Relation to Alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 1035–1045. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.027
- Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., and Smilek, D. (2012). The Unengaged Mind: Defining Boredom in Terms of Attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 482–495. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612456044
- Elpidorou, A. (2018). The Bored Mind Is a Guiding Mind: Toward a Regulatory Theory of Boredom. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 17(3), 455–484. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-017-9515-1
- Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A Flexible Statistical Power Analysis Program for the Social, Behavioral, and Biomedical Sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
- Ferrell, E. L., Watford, T. S., and Braden, A. (2020). Emotion-Regulation Difficulties and Impaired Working Memory Interact to Predict Boredom Emotional Eating. Appetite, 144, 104450. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.104450
- Goldberg, Y. K., Eastwood, J. D., LaGuardia, J., and Danckert, J. (2011). Boredom: An Emotional Experience Distinct from Apathy, Anhedonia, or Depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30(6), 647–666. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2011.30.6.647
- Gross, J. J., and Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion Regulation: Conceptual Foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of Emotion Regulation (pp. 3–24). The Guilford Press.
- Hayes, A. F. (2022). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. The Guilford Press.
- Kil, N., Kim, J., Park, J., and Lee, C. (2021). Leisure Boredom, Leisure Challenge, Smartphone Use, and Emotional Distress among U.S. College Students: Are They Interrelated? Leisure Studies, 40(6), 779–792. https://doi.org/10.1080/02614367.2021.1931414
- Larson, R. W., and Richards, M. H. (1991). Boredom in the Middle School Years: Blaming Schools versus Blaming Students. American Journal of Education, 99(4), 418–443. https://doi.org/10.1086/443992
- LePera, N. (2011). Relations Between Boredom Proneness, Mindfulness, Anxiety, Depression and Substance Use. The New School Psychology Bulletin, 8(2), 15–25.
- Mercer, K. B., and Eastwood, J. D. (2010). Is Boredom Associated with Problem Gambling Behaviour? It Depends on What You Mean by “Boredom”. International Gambling Studies, 10(1), 91–104. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459791003754414
- Martin, M. M., and Rubin, R. B. (1995). A New Measure of Cognitive Flexibility. Psychological Reports, 76(2), 623–626. https://doi.org.subzero.lib.uoguelph.ca/10.2466/pr0.1995.76.2.623
- Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., and Hall, N. C. (2011). Coping with Boredom in School: An Experience Sampling Perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36(1), 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.003
- Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2005). The Cognitive Control of Emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010
- Perone, S., Weybright, E. H., and Anderson, A. J. (2019). Over and over Again: Changes in Frontal EEG Asymmetry across a Boring Task. Psychophysiology, 56(10). https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13427
- Qin, X. (2024). Sample Size and Power Calculations for Causal Mediation Analysis: A Tutorial and Shiny App. Behavior Research Methods, 56(3), 1738–1769. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-023-02118-0
- Richmond, S., Hasking, P., and Meaney, R. (2017). Psychological Distress and Non-Suicidal Self-Injury: The Mediating Roles of Rumination, Cognitive Reappraisal, and Expressive Suppression. Archives of Suicide Research, 21(1), 62–72. https://doi.org/10.1080/13811118.2015.1008160
- Struk, A. A., Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A., and Danckert, J. (2017). A Short Boredom Proneness Scale: Development and Psychometric Properties. Assessment, 24(3), 346–359. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191115609996
- Wegner, D. M., and Erber, R. (1992). The Hyperaccessibility of Suppressed Thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(6), 903–912. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.903
- Weybright, E. H., Doering, E. L., and Perone, S. (2022). Difficulties with Emotion Regulation during COVID-19 and Associations with Boredom in College Students. Behavioral Sciences, 12(8), 296. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080296
- Wolff, W., Bieleke, M., Englert, C., Bertrams, A., Schüler, J., and Martarelli, C. S. (2022). A Single Item Measure of Self-Control – Validation and Location in a Nomological Network of Self-Control, Boredom, and if-then Planning. Social Psychological Bulletin, 17, 1–22. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.7453
- Zhao, T., Ye, L., Hu, Z., and Fu, Z. (2022). A Serial Mediation Model of the Relationship between Suppression Emotion-Regulation Tendency and Outcomes of MOOC Learning by Chinese University Students: The Role of Cognitive Appraisals, Boredom, and Behavioral Avoidance. Computers and Education, 187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2022.104549