No. 1 (2023)
Articles

Being Bored, Happy or Focused – Which Is Best for Creative Thinking? How Different Emotional States Influence Creativity

Anke Zeißig
Brandenburgische Technische Universität Cottbus-Senftenberg
Bio
Sebastian Pannasch
Technische Universität Dresden
Bio

Published 2023-03-09

Keywords

  • Boredom,
  • Creativity,
  • Emotion Induction,
  • Concentration,
  • Joy,
  • Video
  • ...More
    Less

How to Cite

Zeißig, A., & Pannasch, S. (2023). Being Bored, Happy or Focused – Which Is Best for Creative Thinking? How Different Emotional States Influence Creativity. Journal of Boredom Studies, (1). Retrieved from https://www.boredomsociety.com/jbs/index.php/journal/article/view/16

Abstract

We aim to extend the body of research on boredom as a potentially creativity-enhancing state. Therefore, 124 students were assigned to one of five 6-minute interventions (boredom-discomfort, boredom-equanimity, boredom-continuation, joy, and concentration) and the effects on figural as well as verbal fluency and diversity as measures of creativity were examined. It was verified whether the emotional state changed during the intervention. In addition, the emotional dimensions, valence, arousal, and alertness were controlled before and after the test. Boredom-discomfort, joy, and concentration altered the emotion experienced during the intervention in the intended way. The boredom-equanimity and boredom-continuation groups served as control conditions for various boredom states, and less boredom resulted for subjects in these groups. Figural and verbal measures of creativity were differently influenced by the interventions. For verbal fluency, we obtained a significant interaction between time and group, in particular, the performance differed between the intervention with either concentration, or joy. Verbal creativity decreased after intervention in all groups, most for joy and boredom-discomfort groups and least for concentration. In contrast, figural performance increased in four groups, most for boredom-discomfort but not for concentration. Subsequent analyses revealed significant interaction effects between time and group with respect to both verbal and figural measures of creativity. The interventions had not only short-term effects on subjects' emotions but also, in some cases, a significant longer-term impact on emotion dimensions at the end of the study. After discussing methodological aspects, conclusions are drawn for further research approaches.

References

  1. Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in Context: Update to the Social Psychology of Creativity. Westview Press.
  2. Andreasen, N. C. (2005). The Creating Brain. Dana Press.
  3. Andreasen N. C. (2011). A Journey into Chaos: Creativity and the Unconscious. Mens Sana Monographs, 9(1), 42–53. https://doi.org/10.4103/0973-1229.77424
  4. Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., and Nijstad, B. (2008). A Meta-Analysis of 25 Years of Mood-Creativity Research: Hedonic Tone, Activation or Regulatory Focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134(6), 779–806. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012815
  5. Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W., Franklin, M. S., and Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by Distraction: Mind Wandering Facilitates Creative Incubation. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1117–1122. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612446024
  6. Berger, H. (1929). Über das Elektrenkephalogramm des Menschen. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 87(1), 527–570.
  7. Bilalić, M., and McLeod, P. (2014). Why Good Thoughts Block Better Ones. Scientific American, 310(3), 74–79. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0314-74
  8. Bledow, R., Rosing, K., and Frese, M. (2013). A Dynamic Perspective on Affect and Creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 56, 432–450. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2010.0894
  9. Bowden E. M., and Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Aha! Insight Experience Correlates with Solution Activation in the Right Hemisphere. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 730–737. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196539
  10. Brewer, D., Doughtie, E. B., and Lubin, B. (1980). Induction of Mood and Mood Shift. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36(1), 215–226.
  11. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(198001)36:1<215::aid-jclp2270360127>3.0.co;2-6
  12. Byron, K., and Khazanchi, S. (2011). A Meta-Analytic Investigation of the Relationship of State and Trait Anxiety to Performance on Figural and Verbal Creative Tasks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(2), 269–283. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210392788
  13. Chermahini, S. A., and Hommel, B. (2012). Creative Mood Swings: Divergent and Convergent Thinking Affect Mood in Opposite Ways. Psychological Research, 76, 634–640. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0358-z
  14. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Academic Press.
  15. Conner, T. S., and Silvia, P. J. (2015). Creative Days: A Daily Diary Study of Emotion, Personality, and Everyday Creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(4), 463–470. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000022
  16. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2010). Das Flow-Erlebnis: Jenseits von Angst und Langeweile: im Tun aufgehen. Klett-Cotta.
  17. Davis, M. A. (2009). Understanding the Relationship Between Mood and Creativity: A Meta-Analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108(1), 25–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.04.001
  18. De Bono, E. (1992). Laterales Denken. ECON.
  19. Dijksterhuis, A., and Meurs, T. (2006). Where Creativity Resides: The Generative Power of Unconscious Thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(1), 135–146.
  20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.04.007
  21. Doehlemann, M. (1991). Langeweile? Deutung eines verbreiteten Phänomens. Suhrkamp.
  22. Düker, H., Lienert, G.A., Lukesch, H., and Mayrhofer, S. (2001). KLT-R. Konzentrations-Leistungs-Test (revidierte Fassung). Hogrefe.
  23. Eastwood, J. D., and Gorelik, D. (2019). Boredom Is a Feeling of Thinking and a Double-Edged Sword. In J. Ros Velasco (Ed.), Boredom Is in Your Mind (pp. 55-70). Springer.
  24. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A Flexible Statistical Power Analysis Program for the Social, Behavioral, and Biomedical Sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146
  25. Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Benedek, M., and Neubauer, A. C. (2006). Divergent Thinking Training Is Related to Frontal Electroencephalogram Alpha Synchronization. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23(8), 2241–2246. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04751.x
  26. Fink, A., Benedek, M., Grabner, R. H., Staudt, B., and Neubauer, A. C. (2007). Creativity Meets Neuroscience: Experimental Tasks for the Neuroscientific Study of Creative Thinking. Methods, 42(1), 68–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ymeth.2006.12.001
  27. Fink, A., and Benedek, M. (2014). EEG Alpha Power and Creative Ideation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 111–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.002
  28. Fink, A., Graif, B., and Neubauer, A. C. (2009). Brain Correlates Underlying Creative Thinking: EEG Alpha Activity in Professional vs. Novice Dancers. NeuroImage, 46(3), 854–862. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.036
  29. Fink, A., Schwab, D., and Papousek, I. (2011). Sensitivity of EEG Upper Alpha Activity to Cognitive and Affective Creativity Interventions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 82, 233–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.003
  30. Flaherty, A. W. (2005). Frontotemporal and Dopaminergic Control of Idea Generation and Creative Drive. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493(1), 147–153. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.20768
  31. Forgeard, M. J. C. (2011). Happy People Thrive on Adversity: Pre-Existing Mood Moderates the Effect of Emotion Inductions on Creative Thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(8), 904–909. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.07.015
  32. Gasper, K., and Middlewood, B. L. (2014). Approaching Novel Thoughts: Understanding Why Elation and Boredom Promote Associative Thought More than Distress and Relaxation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 50–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.12.007
  33. Gilhooly, K. J., Georgiou, G., and Devery, U. (2013). Incubation and Creativity: Do Something Different. Thinking & Reasoning, 19(2), 137–149.
  34. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2012.749812
  35. Gilman, T. L., Shaheen, R., Nylocks, K. M., Halachoff, D., Chapman, J., Flynn, J. J., Matt, L. M., and Coifman, K. G. (2017). A Film Set for the Elicitation of Emotion in Research: A Comprehensive Catalog Derived from Four Decades of Investigation. Behavior Research Methods, 49(6), 2061–2082. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0842-x
  36. Glass, G. V., Peckham, P. D., and Sanders, J. R. (1972). Consequences of Failure to Meet Assumptions Underlying the Fixed Effects Analyses of Variance and Covariance. Review of Educational Research, 42(3), 237–288. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543042003
  37. Glaveanu, V. P., Hanchett Hanson, M., Baer, J., Barbot, B., Clapp, E. P., Corazza, G. E., Hennessey, B., Kaufman, J. C., Lebuda, I., Lubart, T., Montuori, A., Ness, I. J., Plucker, J., Reiter‐Palmon, R., Sierra, Z., Simonton, D. K., Neves‐Pereira, M. S., and Sternberg, R. J. (2020). Advancing Creativity Theory and Research: A Socio-Cultural Manifesto. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 54(3), 741–745. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.395
  38. Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Hall, N. C., Nett, U. E., Pekrun, R., and Lipnevich, A. A. (2014). Types of Boredom: An Experience Sampling Approach. Motivation and Emotion, 38(3), 401–419. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9385-y
  39. Gross, J. J., and Levenson, R. W. (1995). Emotion Elicitation Using Films. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 87–108. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939508408966
  40. Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5(9), 444–454. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0063487
  41. Guilford, J. P. (1967). Creativity: Yesterday, Today and Tomorrow. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 1(1), 3-14. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.1967.tb00002.x
  42. Haager, J. S., Kuhbandner, C., and Pekrun, R. (2018). To Be Bored or Not to Be Bored – How Task‐Related Boredom Influences Creative Performance. Journal of Creative Behavior, 52(4), 297–304. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.154
  43. Hunter, J. (2015). The Inspiration of Boredom: An Investigation of the Relationship Between Boredom and Creativity [Unpublished Master Thesis] York University, Canada.
  44. Holland, J. L. (1985). The Self-Directed-Search. Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources.
  45. Horn, A., Ostwald, D., Reisert, M., and Blankenburg, F. (2014). The Structural-Functional Connectome and the Default Mode Network of the Human Brain. NeuroImage, 102(Pt1), 142–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.069
  46. Israel, L., Paukner, P., Schiestel, L., Diepold, K., and Schönbrodt, F. (2021). The OpenLAV Video Database for Affect Induction: Analyzing the Uniformity of Video Stimuli Effects [Data set]. https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.5043
  47. Ivcevic, Z., and Hoffmann, J. (2017). Emotions and Creativity: From States to Traits and Emotion Abilities. In G. J. Feist, R. Reiter-Palmon and J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity and Personality Research (pp. 187–213). Cambridge University Press.
  48. Jäger, A. O., Süß, H.-M., and Beauducel, A. (1997). Berliner Intelligenzstruktur-Test. BIS-test, Form 4. Hogrefe.
  49. Janke, W., and Weyers, P. (2008). Positive Emotionen. In W. Janke, M. Schmidt-Daffy and G. Debus (Eds.), Experimentelle Emotionspsychologie (pp. 225–279). Lengerich Pabst.
  50. Jäncke, L. (2012). Funktionale links-rechts-Asymmetrien. In H. O. Karnath and P. Thier (Eds.), Kognitive Neurowissenschaften (pp. 693–703). Springer.
  51. Jaussi, K. S., Knights, A. R., and Gupta, A. (2017). Feeling Good, Being Intentional, and Their Relationship to Two Types of Creativity at Work. Creativity Research Journal, 29(4), 377–386. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2017.1376498
  52. Jaynes, J. (1990). The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind. Houghton Mifflin.
  53. Kim, K. H. (2011). The Creativity Crisis: The Decrease in Creative Thinking Scores on the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 23(4), 285–295. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2011.627805
  54. Kim, K.H., and Pierce, R.A. (2013). Convergent Versus Divergent Thinking. In E. G. Carayannis (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Creativity, Invention, Innovation and Entrepreneurship (pp. 245–250). Springer.
  55. Kast, V. (2003). Vom Interesse und dem Sinn der Langeweile. dtv.
  56. Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG Alpha Oscillations: The Inhibition-Timing Hypothesis. Brain Research Reviews, 53(1), 63–88.
  57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003
  58. Kris, E. (1952). Psychoanalytic Explorations in Art. International Universities Press.
  59. Laufs, H., Kleinschmidt, A., Beyerle, A., Eger, E., Salek-Haddadi, A., Preibisch, C., and Krakow, K. (2003). EEG-Correlated fMRI of Human Alpha Activity. NeuroImage, 19(4), 1463–1476. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00286-6
  60. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. Oxford University Press.
  61. Lucas, B., and Spencer, E. (2017). Teaching Creative Thinking: Developing Learners Who Generate Ideas and Can Think Critically. Crown House Publishing.
  62. Mann, S., and Cadman, R. (2014). Does Being Bored Make Us More Creative? Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 165–173. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2014.901073
  63. Markey, A., Chin, A., Vanepps, E. M., and Loewenstein, G. (2014). Identifying a Reliable Boredom Induction. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 119(1), 237–253.
  64. https://doi.org/10.2466/27.PMS.119c18z6
  65. Marope, M., Griffin, P., and Gallagher, C. (2018). Future Competences and Future of Curriculum. A Global Reference for Curricula Transformation. IBE UNESCO. http://www.ibe.unesco.org/sites/default/files/resources/future_competences_and_the_future_of_curriculum.pdf
  66. Martindale, C. (1989). Personality, Situation, and Creativity. In J. A. Glover, R. R. Ronning and C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of Creativity. Perspectives on Individual Differences (pp. 211–232). Springer.
  67. Martindale, C., and Hasenfus, N. (1978). EEG Differences as a Function of Creativity, Stage of the Creative Process, and Effort to Be Original. Biological Psychology, 6(3), 157–167. https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(78)90018-2
  68. Martindale, C. (1995). Creativity and Connectionism. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward and R. A. Finke (Eds.), The Creative Cognition Approach (pp. 249–268). The MIT Press.
  69. Mastria, S., Agnoli, S., and Corazza, G. E. (2019). How Does Emotion Influence the Creativity Evaluation of Exogenous Alternative Ideas? PLoS ONE, 14(7), e0219298. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219298
  70. Mednick, S. (1962). The Associative Basis of the Creative Process. Psychological Review, 69(3), 220–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048850
  71. Mendelsohn, G. A. (1976). Associative and Attentional Processes in Creative Performance. Journal of Personality, 44(2), 341–369. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1976.tb00127.x
  72. Mölle, M., Marshall, L., Wolf, B., Fehm, H. L., and Born, J. (1999). EEG Complexity and Performance Measures of Creative Thinking. Psychophysiology, 36(1), 95-104. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0048577299961619
  73. OECD (2017). PISA 2021 Creative Thinking Strategic Advisory Group Report. OECD. https://one.oecd.org/document/EDU/PISA/GB(2017)19/en/pdf
  74. OECD (2019). PISA 2021 Creative Thinking Framework Third Draft. OECD. https://www.oecd.org/pisa/publications/PISA-2021-creative-thinking-framework.pdf
  75. Portney, L. G., and Watkins, M. P. (1993). Foundations of Clinical Research: Applications to Practice. Appleton & Lange.
  76. Quindlen, A. (2002). Doing Nothing Is Something. Newsweek, 139(19), 76.
  77. Richter, P., and Hacker, W. (2008). Belastung und Beanspruchung: Stress, Ermüdung und Burnout im Arbeitsleben (2. Auflage). Asanger Verlag.
  78. Ritter, S. M., and Dijksterhuis, A. (2014). Creativity – The Unconscious Foundations of the Incubation Period. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(25), 215.
  79. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00215
  80. Rottenberg, J., Ray, R. D., and Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion Elicitation Using Films. In J. A. Coan and J. J. B. Allen (Eds.), The Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment (pp. 9–28). Oxford University Press.
  81. Runco, M. A. (2017). Comments on Where the Creativity Research Has Been and Where Is it Going. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51(4), 308–313. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.189
  82. Runco, M. A., and Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The Standard Definition of Creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1), 92–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2012.650092
  83. Runco, M. A., and Sakamoto, S. O. (1999). Experimental Studies of Creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of Creativity (pp. 62–92). Cambridge University Press.
  84. Runco, M. A., Acar, S., and Cayirdag, N. (2017). A Closer Look at the Creativity Gap and Why Students Are Less Creative at School than Outside of School. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 24, 242–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.04.003
  85. Salkind, N. J. (Ed.). (2010). Encyclopedia of Research Design. SAGE.
  86. Samson, A. C., Kreibig, S. D., Soderstrom, B., Wade, A. A., and Gross J. J. (2016). Eliciting Positive, Negative and Mixed Emotional States: A Film Library for Affective Scientists. Cognition and Emotion, 30(5), 827–856. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1031089
  87. Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Pecherstorfer, T., Freunberger, R., and Hanslmayr, S. (2005). EEG Alpha Synchronization and Functional Coupling During Top-Down Processing in a Working Memory Task. Human Brain Mapping, 26(2), 148–155.
  88. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20150
  89. Schaefer, A., Nils, F., Sanchez, X., and Philippot, P. (2010). Assessing the Effectiveness of a Large Database of Emotion-Eliciting Films: A New Tool for Emotion Researchers. Cognition & Emotion, 24(7), 1153–1172. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903274322
  90. Schleicher, R. (2009). Emotionen und Peripherphysiologie. Emotionale Filmclips, kontinuierliche Selbstbewertung und peripherphysiologie inklusive okulomotorische Veränderungen. Pabst Science Publishers.
  91. Schuler, H., and Görlich, Y. (2007). Kreativität: Ursachen, Messung, Förderung und Umsetzung in Innovation. Hogrefe.
  92. Scott, G., Leritz, L. E., and Mumford, M. D. (2004). The Effectiveness of Creativity Training: A Quantitative Review. Creativity Research Journal, 16(4), 361–388.
  93. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1604_1
  94. Shofty, B., Gonen, T., Bergmann, E., Mayseless, N., Korn, A., Shamay-Tsoory, S., Grossman, R., Jalon, I., Kahn, I., and Ram, Z. (2022). The Default Network Is Causally Linked to Creative Thinking. Molecular Psychiatry, 27(3), 1848–1854. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-021-01403-8
  95. Shrout, P. E., and Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass Correlations: Uses in Assessing Rater Reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 420–428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420
  96. Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2009). Mind-Wandering. In T. Bayne, A. Cleermans and P. Wilken (Eds.), The Oxford Companion to Consciousness (pp. 443–445). Oxford University Press.
  97. Smart, R. G. (1966). Subject Selection Bias in Psychological Research. Canadian Psychologist/Psychologie Canadienne, 7a(2), 115–121. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0083096
  98. Stein, M. I. (1953). Creativity and Culture. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 36, 311–322. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1953.9712897
  99. Sternberg. R. J. (2007). Creativity as a Habit. In A.-G. Tan (Ed.), Creativity: A Handbook for Teachers (pp. 3–25). World Scientific.
  100. Sternberg, R. J., and Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the Crowd: Cultivating Creativity in a Culture of Conformity. Free Press.
  101. Stevens, C. E., Jr., and Zabelina, D. L. (2019). Creativity Comes in Waves: An EEG-Focused Exploration of the Creative Brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 154–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.02.003
  102. Steyer, R., Schwenkmezger, P., Notz, P., and Eid, M. (1997). Der Mehrdimensionale. Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (MDBF). Handanweisung. Hogrefe.
  103. Süß, H. M., and Beauducel, A. (2015). Modeling the Construct Validity of the Berlin Intelligence Structure Model. Estudos de Psicologia (Campinas), 32, 13–25. https://doi.org/10.1590/0103-166X2015000100002
  104. Tomasino, D. (2007). The Psychophysiological Basis of Creativity and Intuition: Accessing ‘The Zone’ of Entrepreneurship. International Journal of Entrepreneurship and Small Business, 4(5), 528–542. https://doi.org/10.1504/IJESB.2007.014388
  105. Valgeirsdottir, D., and Onarheim, B. (2017). Studying Creativity Training Programs: A Methodological Analysis. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26, 430–439. https://doi.org/10.1111/caim.12245
  106. Williams, R., Runco, M. A., and Berlow, E. (2016). Mapping the Themes, Impact, and Cohesion of Creativity Research over the Last 25 Years. Creativity Research Journal, 28(4), 385–394. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2016.1230358
  107. Yang, H., Chattopadhyay, A., Zhang, K., and Dahl, D. W. (2012). Unconscious Creativity: When Can Unconscious Thought Outperform Conscious Thought? Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(4), 573–581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2012.04.002
  108. Zeißig, A. (2018). The Creative Act as a Mode of Research: Ten Emotions – Ten Films. Neuhaus.
  109. Zempelin, S., Sejunaite, K., Lanza, C., and Riepe, M. (2021). Emotion Induction in Young and Old Persons on Watching Movie Segments: Facial Expressions Reflect Subjective Ratings. PLoS ONE, 16. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253378
  110. Zhong, C. B., Dijksterhuis, A., and Galinsky, A. D. (2008). The Merits of Unconscious Thought in Creativity. Psychological Science, 19(9), 912–918. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02176.x
  111. Zhu, C. and Zhang, L.-F. (2011). Thinking Styles and Conceptions of Creativity among University Students. Educational Psychology, 31(3), 361–375.
  112. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2011.557044