Journal of Boredom
Studies (ISSN 2990-2525)
Issue 1, 2023, pp. 1–33
https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7144313
https://www.boredomsociety.com/jbs
The Significance of
Boredom: A Literature Review
Mariusz Finkielsztein
Collegium Civitas,
Poland
mariusz.finkielsztein@gmail.com
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1620-9402
How to cite this paper: Finkielsztein,
M. (2023). The Significance of Boredom: A Literature Review. Journal
of Boredom Studies, 1.
https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7144313
Abstract: This article aims at providing concise but
thorough presentation of the state of art in the emerging field of boredom
studies evidencing the significance of boredom. The premise of the significance
of boredom is to be expounded by documenting its widespread, social
consequences, functions and positive outcomes. Boredom has
been found prevalent irrespectively of age, gender, culture or social class. It
affects all main spheres of human life – work, leisure, education, romantic
relationships, and even religious life. It has also been evidenced that boredom
has many significant consequences. It has been associated with, among others,
risk-taking behaviours, overeating, impulse shopping, or (self-)destructive and
violent behaviours. Yet, boredom may serve numerous significant functions as
well. As an emotion, it is important for cognition, motivation and
communication and has had evolutionary meaning for human beings. In society
nowadays, it serves as a defensive mechanism against overload of stimuli, but
somehow to the contrary is also found to be a basic mechanism animating current
consumerism. Boredom is also conceived to be a catalyst for reflection,
self-cognition, creativity, and as a consequence a
rudimentary element of culture production and its advances.
Keywords: boredom, emotions,
boredom studies, significance of boredom, interdisciplinary, functions of
boredom.
1. Introduction: Underestimation of
Boredom
A sacramental saying, repeated over and over again in almost all publications on
the subject, specifies that boredom is a widespread and fairly prevalent
phenomenon, yet still not ubiquitous enough to become a fully normalised
subject for scholarship in many disciplines. Although in some subdisciplines boredom
has already long been a legitimate topic of investigation, yet, generally in
the academic context, the subject itself seems to still be regarded as
intriguing but slightly whimsical, “weird, crazy or unworthy of study”
(Eastwood, quoted in Rhodes, 2015, p. 278). It is still being
disregarded by many as a respectable and recognised subject of scientific
endeavour (see more in Finkielsztein, 2021, pp. 15–44). As Randy Malamud (2016) recollected: “When I told colleagues that I was travelling 5,000 miles
to attend a conference on boredom, the first reaction was, inevitably, a
sardonic chuckle.” The prevailing opinion on the idea of studying boredom is
that it looks like a leisure activity for bored academics with no serious
issues to reflect on. It is “a relatively minor irritation” (Conrad, 1997, p. 474), a “minor affect” (as opposed to the more clearly-defined “major
affects” of hate, lust, etc.; Ngai, 2005, p. 8) a
“mild psychic disturbance” that “can hardly be the purview of a rigorous social
science concerned with altogether weightier issues, and the reassurance of
dealing with such solid, measurable facts as income disparities or the rate of
violent crime” (Gardiner, 2012, p. 38).
Moreover,
boredom “like normality, is a taken-for-granted part of everyday life” (Misztal, 2016, p. 109; cf. Barbalet,
1999, p. 633), it “is generally paid scant and
superficial attention, passed over lightly as transitory and insignificant”
(Healy, 1984, p. 9) as most people “do not fully
acknowledge or […] are not fully conscious of what a grave affliction boredom
is” (Fromm, 1986, p. 14). This includes many
scholars as well; for instance, Reinhard Kuhn (1976), who in
his erudite analysis of the notion of ennui in Western literature tradition
totally dismissed everyday boredom as worthy of scientific attention. Boredom is still usually not considered a part of the basic
curriculum of any discipline, as “there are no courses [on boredom] offered at
the universities, apart from the fact that one is often bored during one’s
studies” (Svendsen, 2005, p. 18) and the limited exceptions confirm that tendency rather than
contradict it.
This article aims at evidencing the significance of boredom and
providing researchers from various disciplines and academic journalists with
argumentation that boredom matters and constitutes a phenomenon worth
exploring. It seems to me that this may also be a good way to propagate and
develop the idea of boredom studies (Gardiner and Haladyn,
2016). The premise of the significance
of boredom is to be expounded by documenting its widespread, social
consequences, functions and positive outcomes. There are of course limits to
what is evidenced in this paper, yet, I chose to focus
on advocating the thesis of the significance of boredom as so many
automatically and without further reflection contend something opposite.
2. A Serious Issue
In
contradiction to a general disregard to boredom, many authors, even prominent
ones, have considered boredom to be a serious matter, “a central
twenty-first-century problem” (Avramenko, 2004, p. 108), “a major social problem” (Klapp, 1986, p. 26), “an inherent part of the human being” (Ros Velasco, 2017, p. 184), one of the greatest
miseries of humankind (Fromm, 2011; Nisbet, 1983) and “that part of hell which Dante
forgot to describe in La Divina Commedia” (Casanova, quoted in Bergler, 1945, p. 38) –
a species of ‘psychic pain’ (Wallace, 2011). The
science fiction writer, Isaac Asimov (1964) even
predicted that in 2014 the “disease of boredom,” having “serious mental,
emotional and sociological consequences,” will constitute one of the most
severe sufferings haunting humankind. Boredom is positioned with such serious phenomena as
‘alienation,’ ‘anomie,’ ‘disenchantment’ and/or ‘depression’ (Irvine, 2001) and is believed to be the quality that
makes us human (Kolakowski, 1999), thus constituting an inevitable part of
human nature.
Boredom is also claimed to be
significant because, as according to Walter Benjamin, who summarised Émile
Tardieu’s (1913) book on
the subject, “all human activity is shown to be a vain attempt to escape from
boredom, but in which, at the same time, everything that was, is, and will be
appears as the inexhaustible nourishment of that feeling” (2002, p.
102). Similar claims have been made by many well-known authors, inter alia by
German philosophers Arthur Schopenhauer and Martin Heidegger; German-born
American social psychologist, Erich Fromm; French philosopher, Claude Adrien Helvétius; French poet, Charles Baudelaire; French
philosopher and novelist, Albert Camus; or American writer, David Foster
Wallace; who all indicated in one way or another that “most of us spend nearly
all our time and energy trying to distract ourselves from feeling” (Wallace, 2011, p. 85).
In this vein, Schopenhauer suggested that boredom was the foundation of all
religions – “[m]an creates for himself in his own image demons, gods, and
saints; then to these must be incessantly offered sacrifices, prayers, temple
decorations, vows and their fulfilment, pilgrimages, salutations, adornment of
images and so on” (1969, p. 323; cf. Helvétius,
1810). Mihalyi Csikszentmihalyi (2000),
American social psychologist, the creator of the theory of flow (optimal
experience) explained that an understanding of boredom is of central importance
to all “interested in enhancing the quality of life” (p. 444) because it is one
of the main disturbances to a person’s well-being.
In general, people are believed to
be led by imperative towards activity, and by a fear of boredom, which, as
Bertrand Russell, British philosopher and mathematician, a Nobel prize
laureate, claimed is “one of the great
motive powers throughout the historical epoch” (1932, p. 57)
and which effect “on a large scale in history is underestimated” (Inge, 1940, p.
386). Scrutinising the relevant literature suggests that many agree that
boredom is an essential incentive for social change and (r)evolution, and,
thus, for the historical process. As Tardieu (1913, pp. 195, 283)
indicated, “the infinite evolution of societies, their progress and decay,
express their eternal boredom” and “boredom, which is the sting that
precipitates the race of this world, will never be blunted.” Boredom is
credited with the rise and decline of civilisations, heresies, reformation, the
rise of nationalism and radical political movements (e.g., the Nazis), and all
kinds of revolutions, terrorism and wars (Inge, 1940; Kuhn, 1976; Kustermans and Ringmar, 2011; Laugesen, 2012; Maeland and Brunstad, 2009; Moravia, 1965; Tochilnikova, 2020).
3. Prevalence of
Boredom
It has already become a
boring platitude that boredom is “one of the most unexpectedly common of all
human emotions” (Toohey, 2011, p. 1). There are frequent abstract
claims of the vast proliferation of boredom in modern society (e.g., Klapp, 1986; Svendsen, 2005; Tardieu, 1913; Toohey,
2011), and many people, including scholars and
writers, have believed that it is an inevitable part of human life and
condition, that it “is an inescapable fact like the illness that comes in its
time” (Tardieu, 1913, p. 233). George Byron (2006) in his Don Juan even suggested that “Society is now one polish’d horde,/Form’d of two
mighty tribes,/the Bores and Bored” (XLV, 94–95; cf. Kierkegaard, 1843). Although on the intuitional level such
claims might be seen as correct, however, it is still essential to prove them,
at least, partially based on the scientific literature. Alycia Chin et al. (2017) found that 63 per cent of the participants in
a US-based sample (n=3,867) reported boredom at least once over the study
period (7–10 days chosen randomly in a period of two years). Respondents
answered a set of short questions, including one about their emotional state,
every half-an-hour, via a custom-made iPhone app (participants without an
iPhone were provided with one). Boredom was recorded in 2.8 per cent of all
half-hour reports and was the seventh most frequently reported out of 17
emotional measures. Occasional occurrences of boredom were confirmed in this
massive, although not representative, gender- and age-balanced national sample.
Another argument in favour of claims about the ubiquity
of boredom is the fact that it is found among representatives of all social
classes. Traditionally, boredom was conceived to be a characteristic of social
elites, the leisure classes, for whom boredom was both a privilege and a sign
of social position (Bernstein, 1975; Healy, 1984; Lepenies, 1992; Scitovsky, 1999; Tardieu,
1913; Van den Berg and O’Neill, 2017; Veblen, 2007). Kings
(Kuhn, 1976; Pascal, 1910) and nobles, such as the French aristocracy in Versailles (Saint-Simon,
1902), or the Polish baronage (Tazbir, 1997), were bored. Some, like Virginia
Woolf, even differentiated upper class boredom (ennui, melancholia, spleen) from the common boredom of the
lower classes (quoted in Crangle, 2008, p.
217). The boredom of the non-privileged classes has a less noble tradition than
that so eloquently described as the boredom of the wealthy (frequently written
down by the victims themselves), yet many studies has already shown that
boredom is an everyday experience of the working class (e.g., Davies, 1926; Grubb, 1975), unemployed (Jahoda et al., 2009), homeless (Marshall et al., 2019; O’Neill, 2014, 2017), refugees (Chan and Loveridge, 1987; Wagner and
Finkielsztein, 2021), and the citizens of marginalised
poor countries (see the cases of Ethiopia [Mains, 2007], Egypt [Schielke, 2008]; Niger [Masquelier, 2013, 2019]; South
Africa [Tournadre, 2020];
Georgia [Frederiksen, 2013, 2017]) or
minorities (for example, the case of American native population [Jervis et al.,
2003]). Representatives of all social
strata are thus not immune to boredom, even if the particular
reasons are not identical for all social classes.
Boredom also seems to be experienced independently of
individual innate qualities. In many studies men are found to experience
boredom more frequently, be more prone to boredom (e.g. Chin et al., 2017; Farmer and Sundberg, 1986; Vodanovich et al., 2011; Wilson et al., 2014), and be more sensation-seeking (Zuckerman, 1979) than women, which is speculated to be an
innate, evolutionary-based tendency, however, some studies found that girls are
more severely exposed to boredom, due to greater social/cultural constraints,
especially in their leisure time (Patterson et al., 2000; Wegner et al., 2006) but are less likely to admit to the feeling
when asked. Many studies have not found any statistically significant
correlation between boredom and gender, but all shows that boredom is reported
by both sexes.
Boredom is also experienced irrespective of age. Children
are bored due to all kinds of constraints (school regulations, parents’ rules,
bad weather, illness), or when lacking parental attention, and feeling lonely
(Jackson, 1990; Kirova, 2004; Phillips, 1993). Teenagers are the
quintessentially bored age group (de Chenne, 1998; Esman, 1979; Farnworth, 1998; Sundberg and Bisno, 1983), forging and seeking their identity,
rebelling against adult regulations (e.g., via vandalism and/or delinquency),
and they apply boredom as a selection mechanism of what is worth pursuing in
their further life and what is not. Adults, although not usually perceived as a
group at risk of boredom, are far from being immune to it, as, for instance,
research into boredom at work may confirm. Boredom is also noted as a
characteristic experience of middle-age crisis (Bernstein, 1975; Martin et al., 2006), when someone feels a sense of failure and
disappointment with their life, mixed with the comfort, security and relative
affluence that makes it predictable, monotonous and, for the most part,
unchallenging. Chronic boredom that has long been latent is revealed and
emerges into awareness, prompting, inter alia, adultery, divorce and
remarriage. The feeling is also suggested as a characteristic of senility,
especially in retirement (Hoeyberghs et al., 2018). People who are no longer actively engaged in
work face “the problem of enforced downtime” (Mann, 2016), or
boredom caused by one’s addiction to work – this is why so many pensioners
decide to work after retirement, launching ‘encore careers.’
Boredom, despite starting its ‘career’ as a strictly
European/Western concept, seems to become more and more globalised as the basic
mechanisms responsible for its aetiology were popularised due to the processes
of modernisation that have affected almost all parts of the globe. Generally,
there is a scarcity of cross-cultural research on boredom, and almost all of
them have been primarily focused on the differences in boredom proneness
between students from different countries/cultures (Ng et al., 2015; Sundberg et al., 1991; Vodanovich and Watt, 1999; Vodanovich et al., 2011). Irrespective of particular
correlational measurements, the notion of boredom was commonly
recognised in all compared countries (USA, Australia, Canada, German, China and
Lebanon). Most languages nowadays have, or have adapted, some expression(s) for
boredom, as confirmed by the Wikipedia entry for ‘boredom,’ which exists in 55
languages so distant as Chinese, Yiddish, Indonesian, or Arabic, and in some
anthropological data (Musharbash, 2007).
Boredom affects the main spheres of human life – work,
leisure, education, romantic relationships and even religious life. One third
of Britons admitted to being bored at work for most of the day (Development
Dimensions International, 2004), boredom was declared by 50 per
cent of those employed in the financial services sector (Mann, 2007), and 52 per cent of US employees in a national Gallup poll conceded
that they were ‘not engaged’ and 18 per cent even ‘actively disengaged’ at work
(Newport, 2013). Boredom was also found to be the
second most commonly suppressed emotion at work (Mann,
1999). Another study has shown that almost
one-third of surveyed employees spent approximately two hours daily pursuing
private affairs at work because they were bored, which can be calculated in
terms of lost benefits for employers and the economy (Malachowski,
2005). The outcome of workplace boredom
is, therefore, so called ‘empty labour’ (Paulsen, 2015) –
appropriating time that officially belongs to the employer by constantly
engaging in non-work-related activities at work.
The
literature on work characteristics and the emotional life of employees has
identified boredom as a quality of some occupations. Traditionally, jobs such
as factory workers (Davies, 1926; Grubb, 1975; Hill, 1975; Kerce, 1985; Nichols and Beynon, 1977; Thackray, 1981), clerks (Baker, 1992;
Dyer-Smith and Wesson, 1995; Lee, 1986) and shop assistants (Fisher, 1987; Mann, 2012) were found to induce boredom. To that group we can also add call
centre staff (Walker, 2009), employees in the catering sector
(Tsai, 2016), and, loosely, nurses (Loukidou, 2008). All these jobs require low or
moderate skill levels, have low recognition or rewards, and involve monotonous
tasks or occasionally no activity while being constrained to stay in a
particular location (behind the cash register, desk, assembly line, on the
ward, etc.).
Another group of boring occupations is that associated
with some kind of isolation from society. This includes such diverse jobs such as
truck drivers (Drory, 1982; McBain,
1970), astronauts (Hancock, 2017; Volante et al., 2016) soldiers (Bartone,
2005; Maeland and Brunstad, 2009) and
prison guards (Shamir and Drory, 1982). All these occupations involve being
alienated from society and being a kind of ‘paid prisoner,’ locked in isolated
units (prison, spacecraft, truck cockpit, military base in a foreign country).
Occupations associated with the possibility of danger,
such as professional soldiers (Bartone, 2005; Fisher, 1987; Harris and Segal, 1985; Maeland and Brunstad, 2009), border
patrol officers, operational intelligence agents (Hancock and Krueger, 2010), security guards (Kerce,
1985), security specialists (Charlton and Hertz, 1989), police officers (Anderson, 2015; Phillips, 2016; Van Maanen, 1974), firefighters (Watt, 2002), or airplane pilots (Graeber, 1989; Grose, 1988; O’Hanlon, 1981) may form another group of boring
occupations. A common description in all these occupations is that they include
long periods of underemployment and only brief moments when the use of high
skills is necessary (emergency conditions). As Grose (1988) noted for airplane pilots, such jobs consist of “endless hours of
tedious boredom punctuated by moments of stark terror” (p. 30), when alertness,
rapid decision-making and high professional skills are essential in order to respond adequately. For the
majority of time, these occupations lack activities that would be
identified as ‘real work’ and consist of more mundane tasks, or simply waiting
for the opportunity to make use of high-quality training. To this category,
although bereft of major personal perils, may be added air traffic controllers
(Langan-Fox et al., 2009; Thackray, 1981), unmanned aerial vehicle (UAV,
drone) operators (Thompson et al., 2006) or any
kind of controllers needed high skills in a case of emergency, which are
underutilised for the majority of the time (Johansson, 1989). Similar group of occupations demonstrated to
include significant levels of boredom involve highly-trained professionals with
a lot of responsibility, such as train engineers (Haga,
1984), anaesthesiologists (Weinger, 1999), surgeons (Csikszentmihalyi, 2000),
therapists (Campagne, 2012; Wangh, 1979) and lawyers (Harper, 1987), who despite their high expertise are often compelled to deal with
unchallenging cases that are far below their skill levels.
Boredom was also noted among creative workers such as
orchestra musicians (Faulkner, 1973; Parasuraman and Purohit, 2000), whose actual job is incongruent
with their education, which prepares them for solo performance and emphasises
creativity, when they are compelled to play under the strict supervision of a
conductor and are simply ‘anonymous cogs’ in the orchestra. Boredom was also
observed among blues, jazz and classical musicians playing commercial gigs,
when, to respond to audience demand, they are compelled to perform a limited
and repetitive repertoire numerous times (Grazian, 2003; Ryan, 2011). Robert Stebbins (1990, p. 116) observed “onstage boredom” (“auto
pilot syndrome”) among Canadian stand-upers, when
they are compelled to perform the same set night after night doing a circuit.
They are overwhelmed by the dullness of daily existence and performances appear
to be a drudgery for them, no longer generating thrill and enthusiasm.
Apart from being ubiquitous at work, boredom also
constitutes a significant leisure experience. Leisure boredom, defined as “the
subjective perception that available leisure experiences are not sufficient to
instrumentally satisfy needs for optimal arousal” (Iso-Ahola
and Weissinger, 1990, p. 4) is no less frequent.
According to the international research of the International Social Survey
Programme (Haller et al., 2013; ISSP Research Group, 2009) people admitted to feeling bored when at leisure (very
often/often/sometimes) in all 36 countries included in the study (the average
amounted to 36%). Boredom is also a significant experience in long-term
relationships (Harasymchuk and Fehr, 2010, 2012, 2013) and sexual life (Tunariu
and Reavey, 2003). Even
religious life seems to induce boredom in many followers (Raposa,
1985), especially those of ‘traditional’ religions,
and the growth of “theatrical evangelism” (Klapp, 1986, p. 18) is a symptom of religious boredom.
Boredom is also noted as experienced in all kinds of
venues and places; in the countryside (Schielke, 2008), metropolis (Aho, 2007; Simmel, 1950) and suburbs (Gamsby,
2012) alike, at the cinema (Misek,
2010; Rhym, 2012; Schaefer, 2003), art gallery (Sontag, 1967), museum (Sánchez-Vázquez, 2004), and
classical ballet (Svendsen, 2016). Boredom is also frequently
experienced in all kinds of total institutions: hospitals (field hospital [Svendsen,
2005]; hospital in convict settlement [Dostoevsky, quoted
in Avramenko, 2004]; mental
institutions [Binnema, 2004; Goffman,
1961; Steele et al., 2013], and rehabilitation centres [Bracke et al., 2006; Bracke and Verhaeghe, 2010]), monasteries (Tardieu, 1913; Wenzel, 1967), prisons (Shalev, 2008), youth confinements (Bengtsson, 2012),
refugee camps (Wagner and Finkielsztein, 2021), and POW [prisoners-of-war] camps
(Laugesen, 2012), and is
noted in a variety of extreme life situations and circumstances, such as living
under German occupation (Czocher, 2018) or in a Jewish ghetto during the Second World War (Korczak,
2003), fighting in a war (Kustermans
and Ringmar, 2011; Maeland
and Brunstad, 2009; Ware, 1986) or sickness and dying (Tolstoy, 1970).
The enormous popularity of that theme in fiction may
serve as one more, indirect proof of the social prevalence and significance of
boredom. The list of novels and plays that include boredom as a significant
issue is both long and prestigious. In one way or another, bored are characters
of inter alia Jane Austen (Emma),
Samuel Becket (Waiting for Godot),
Saul Bellow (Humboldt’s Gift, Dangling Man), Georges Bernanos (The Diary of a Country Priest), George
Byron (Don Juan), Albert Camus (The Plague, The Stranger), René de Chateaubriand (René), Anton Chekhov (Uncle
Vanya, Three Sisters, The Cherry Orchard), Benjamin Constant (Adolphe), Charles Dickens (Bleak House), Denis Diderot (Candide), Fyodor Dostoevsky (Demons), Gustave Flaubert (Madame Bovary), Johann Wolfgang von
Goethe (The Sorrows of Young Werther,
Faust), Ivan Goncharov (Oblomov), Julien Gracq (The Opposing Shore), Michel Houellebecq (The Elementary Particles), Victor Hugo (Les Miserables),
Joris-Carl Huysmans (Against Nature),
Henrik Ibsen (Hedda Gabler, A Doll’s House), Mihail
Lermontov (A Hero of Out Times),
Thomas Mann (The Magic Mountain),
Alberto Moravia (Boredom), Alfons de Musset (The
Confession of a Child of the Century), Alexander Pushkin (Eugene Onegin),
Jean-Paul Sartre (Nausea), Stendhal (Red and Black), Leo Tolstoy (Anna Karenina, The Death of Ivan Ilyich), Ivan Turgenev (Fathers and Sons, Rudin, Diary of a Superfluous Man), David Foster
Wallace (The Pale King) or Oscar Wilde (The Picture of Dorian Gray).
4. Consequences of
Boredom
Boredom also matters
because it has been demonstrated to be the direct or indirect cause of many
behaviours that are perceived as unproductive, destructive, dangerous and/or
pathological. Human beings are believed to be a species particularly addicted
to novelty and new stimuli/information, and boredom seems to be a mechanism
that “motivates people to engage in any activity that seems meaningful to them”
(Van Tilburg and Igou, 2012, p. 182)
at the time. “Boredom is essentially a thwarted desire for events, not
necessarily pleasant ones, but just occurrences such as will enable the victim
of ennui to know one day from another” (Russell, 1932, p. 58).
Many people merely kill/waste their time in order to stave off boredom, instead
of making use of it (Fromm, 1986; Schopenhauer, 1969) – they “do timepass,” engage in activity that
is “neither serious nor productive because it is merely intended to kill time
and ward off potential boredom” (Fuller, 2011, p. 1)
without adding any value or meaning to their lives (Beckelman,
1995; Klapp, 1986) and providing them with only momentary pleasure, a fast endorphin
intake.
Boredom produces an almost irresistible need to escape
the feeling, which can be positive/creative or destructive. The thesis that
people are capable of doing all kinds of things just to alleviate their boredom
is illustrated in a series of experiments on self-administered pain (Havermans et al., 2015; Nederkoorn et al., 2016; Wilson
et al., 2014). Participants were subjected to
boring conditions (e.g., spending 6 to 15 minutes in a room with nothing to do
but think, or watching one short fragment of a documentary over
and over again for an hour), and lacking other options to engage, were
prone to voluntarily self-administering electric shocks to themselves. The
motivational power of the unpleasantness of boredom turned out to be so
significant for some people that they even preferred negative stimuli to
boredom – participants in boring conditions inflicted pain on themselves more
frequently and with higher intensity than those in the control group (Havermans et al., 2015). No
similar effect was found for sadness (Nederkoorn et
al., 2016), suggesting that a tendency to
self-inflict pain was not an answer to a general negative emotional experience,
but to boredom specifically.
In the same vein, boredom was found to be associated with
risk-taking behaviours and ‘edgework’ (Lyng, 2005). People looking for anything to alleviate the feeling engage in all
kinds of activities that may have a negative impact on their well-being, life
prospects or safety (Stebbins, 2003). Boredom proneness was found to be
connected to internet sex addiction (Chaney and Blalock, 2006), hypersexual behaviour (Reid et al., 2011), reckless driving (Dahlen et al., 2005; Kass et al., 2010; Mann, 2012), drunk
driving (Arnett, 1990), and drug and alcohol abuse (Iso-Ahola and Crowley, 1991; LePera, 2011). Bored people are more likely to
experiment with drugs, including alcohol (Krotava and
Todman, 2014), which
may lead to regular use (Corvinelli, 2007; Csikszentmihalyi and Larson, 1978), and Paul Martin (2009) claimed even that Elvis Presley and Janis Joplin, who were both drug
addicts, were among the many victims of boredom. Results of research by Alex Blaszczynsky and colleagues (1990) suggested that a substantive group of
pathological gamblers are motivated by boredom (the rest by depression).
Gambling is employed by many, similarly to drugs or alcohol, as a remedy for
boredom (Martin, 2009; Mercer and Eastwood, 2010). In this capacity, Russian roulette, as a
highly risky gambling game invented in the trenches of the Russo-Turkish war
(1877–1878), is believed to be the result of “a combination of acute boredom
and military inactivity, often aggravated by unreasonable consumption of vodka”
(Maeland and Brunstad, 2009, p. 9).
Binge eating/overeating/polyphagia, i.e., eating more
frequently and/or excessively than needed (found also in animals when bereft of
stimuli [Wemelsfelder, 1985]) is another behaviour connected to boredom.
Research into emotional eating, a change in the consumption of food in response
to emotional stimuli, has shown that feeling bored increases the frequency and
amount of food consumed (Havermans et al., 2015; Koball et al., 2012; Moynihan et al., 2015), and is associated with obesity (Abramson and
Stinson, 1977). Shopping involves a similar
tendency (Tymkiw, 2017) – bored
individuals buy often unnecessary, random things, and engage in compulsive
entertainment more frequently than individuals who are not bored (Martin, 2009). They also more frequently engage in impulse Internet shopping as a
method for ‘clicking the boredom away’ (Sundström et
al., 2019).
Boredom is thus a primary cause of many of human
(self-)destructive behaviours (Fromm, 1973) and
constitutes a significant social problem (Calhoun, 2011). As Søren Kierkegaard (1843)
famously stated, ‘boredom is the root of all evil’ and as Joanna Petry-Mroczkowska (2004) observed “the list of consequences
of boredom surprisingly coincides with the list of deadly sins” (p. 196).
Bertrand Russell (1932) even claimed that “[b]oredom is
therefore a vital problem for the moralist, since at least half the sins of
mankind are caused by the fear of it” (p. 61) – for other half I would
credit boredom itself. There is a vast literature connecting boredom with all
kinds of misbehaviours and crimes – Jeff Ferrell (2004) even
deliberated whether many crimes are “committed not against people or property
as such, but against boredom” (p. 293). Boredom is found to result in drawing
adolescents into religious practices that advocate violence (e.g., Satanism [Clark,
1994]), ignoring legal standards by police officers
(Welsh, 1981), delinquency and vandalism (Bengtsson,
2012; Csikszentmihalyi and Larson, 1978; Newberry and Duncan, 2001; Scitovsky, 1999), public violence and alcohol-related assaults
(Homel et al., 1992) and
murders. In the criminological literature the term ‘thrill killing’ suggests
premeditated murder motivated by the sheer excitement of the act (Branković, 2015), and
many murderers have admitted to killing out of boredom (Clemons, 2013; Vaneigem, 1994), as
reflected in some literary characters, such as Lafcadio
Wluiki (Andre Gide, The Vatican Cellars) or Meursault (Albert Camus, The Stranger).
Boredom is believed to be a significant motivational
factor in all kinds of violent and destructive behaviours, such as massacres
(see the case of the My Lai Massacre in Maeland and Brunstad [2009]),
pogroms (Goebbels boasted at the time of the first Jewish pogroms that “at
least the National Socialists were not boring,” [quoted in Adorno, 2006, p. 237]), terrorism (Hanby, 2004; Tochilnikova, 2020), torture (Kustermans
and Ringmar, 2011), wars (e.g., king of Epirus,
Pyrrhus who launched his military campaigns out of boredom [see Kuhn, 1976; Toohey, 1988]), and riots and revolutions (see
the case of (a) Fronde (1648–1653) [Lepenies, 1992]; (b) French Revolution [Burke, quoted in
Mallory, 2003]; (c) the revolution of 1848 in
France [de Lamartine, quoted in Healy, 1984; Klapp, 1986]; (d) the Paris riots of 1968 [Seeman,
quoted in Klapp, 1986; Vaneigem, 1994], and (e) the London riot of 2011 [Mann,
2016]). Many such events are claimed to result from
profound hopelessness and lack of agency, and a strong inclination to (re)gain
a sense of control and power. Massacres, lynches, torture or acts of terrorism
are not usually performed by pathological sadists but just by powerless,
frustrated, frequently bored (also chronically/existentially bored, see the
state of the Cafard of American
troops in Vietnam [Maeland and Brunstad,
2009]) people who have an opportunity to
gain some power and fight their boredom back, distracting themselves from their
boredom. George Steiner (1971) claimed even that chronic ennui
has contributed to the ‘civilised barbarity’ of two world wars, the Nazi death
camps, and the development and use of weapons of mass destruction. Boredom was
also suggested to be a major factor in voting for charismatic and/or populistic
leaders, thus, in political radicalization nowadays (Tochilnikova,
2020).
Boredom was also found to be a risk factor in heart
disease (Franzmeier, quoted in Brisset
and Snow, 1993), to increase the likelihood of
dying (Britton and Shipley, 2010) and to
be “a key component of psychopathology and neurological disorders” (Goldberg et
al., 2011, p. 662). Boredom, connected to
lower levels of attention, makes people more vulnerable to performance
decrease, which is believed to constitute a real life
threat, for instance, for soldiers (Maeland and Brunstad, 2009). It
also contributes to antipsychotic medication non-adherence – some schizophrenia
patients have positive attitude towards some psychotic symptoms, such as
delusional feelings of importance and power, hearing voices, and the experience
of being another person, that provide them with substantial stimuli that is
absent in a drug-induced reality (Branković, 2015). Boredom was also found to be a major
obstacle in long-lasting romantic relationships and a contributor to relational
problems and divorces (Harasymchuk and Fehr, 2010).
Boredom and boredom proneness were also found positively
correlated to many negative affective states, such as anger and aggression
(Mercer-Lynn et al., 2011, 2013; Rupp and Vodanovich, 1997; Van Tilburg et al., 2019), depression (Farmer and Sundberg, 1986; Goldberg et al., 2011; Malkovsky et al., 2012; Newell et al., 2012), anxiety (Fahlman et al., 2009, 2013; Newell
et al., 2012), hostility (Dahlen et al., 2004), hostility towards outgroups (Van Tilburg and
Igou, 2011), loneliness (Farmer and Sundberg, 1986), hopelessness (Farmer and Sundberg, 1986), alienation and poor interpersonal and social
relationships (Tolor, 1989; Watt and
Vodanovich, 1999), lower job and life satisfaction
(Farmer and Sundberg, 1986; Kass et
al., 2001), stress[1] (Hancock, 2017;
Merrifield and Danckert, 2014), apathy (Bargdill,
2014; Goldberg et al., 2011), retreatism (understood as a state of psychic
passivity [Misztal, 2016]),
fatigue (Loukidou, 2008; Mann, 2016), and frustration (Baker et al., 2010; Hill and
Perkins, 1985).
5. Functions of
Boredom
The significance of
boredom lies in the fact that it serves various functions that used to be, and
still are, beneficial for human beings. Boredom, as an emotion, serves specific
functions, which are (1) cognition, (2) motivation, and (3) communication (Nesse, 1990).
Emotions constitute a mechanism signalling significance, i.e.,
that something important is happening from the point of view of individual
well-being or the tasks carried out by someone. They therefore provide
information about one’s ambience and attitude towards it. Emotions indicate the
status of goal achievement, and boredom, as a negative emotion, informs that
the realisation of a person’s interests is threatened or hindered (Ekman and
Davidson, 1994). It thus constitutes an ‘internal
alarm’ (Elpidorou, 2015) that
alerts/informs/signals/registers that the situation at hand is not
satisfactory, beneficial or meaningful.
Emotions motivate someone to take action in order to
fulfil their goals or avoid negative outcomes (Bench and Lench,
2013), and thus control and direct
behaviour, goal choice, motivational priorities or energy and attention
allocation (Lewis et al., 2008; cf. Stets and Turner, 2006). Emotions are usually raised in situations
requiring adaptation, and are a basic mechanism for
modulating and selecting actions, helping establish new goals, explore
alternatives, seek a change or trigger the motivation to switch goals (Macklem, 2015). Boredom, therefore, is indicated
to form a motivating/energising force, a catalyst for action that ‘pushes’ one
to seek for activity that seems meaningful or interesting, to engage in
challenge-seeking behaviour, and “may
enable a stalled self to get moving, to once again experience the flow and momentum of life” (Brisset
and Snow, 1993, p. 243; see also Beckelman,
1995; Belton
and Priyadharshini, 2007; Bench
and Lench, 2013; Berlyne, 1960; Mann, 2007; Moran, 2003; Van Tilburg and
Igou, 2011). In that capacity, boredom is a defence against meaninglessness (Barbalet, 1999; Van Tilburg and Igou, 2012). Andreas Elpidorou
(2017b) even compared the function of
boredom to that served by pain – although the sensation of pain is unpleasant,
it signals the presence of harm and motivates one to change one’s behaviour, and protects a person. Analogically, boredom both
monitors and regulates behaviour (see a regulatory theory of boredom in Elpidorou, 2017a),
keeping someone in tune with their interests, and preventing them from wasting
time (Johnsen, 2016).
Emotions also serve communication functions, informing
other people about someone’s attitudes, interests, values and/or beliefs.
Boredom is interpreted as the communication of an intention of withdrawal from
a situation/interaction and/or lack of interest in it. It may also serve as an
excuse or justification for non-involvement, and mask laziness or insufficient
cognitive abilities (Mann and Cadman, 2014).
Boredom may be a demonstration of disagreement, resistance against values,
beliefs or actions of others. As Reed Larson and Maryse Richards (1991) suggested, boredom at school “might be
understood less as a spontaneous psychological state and more as the expression
of a value or a posture that students adopt toward schoolwork and school
authority” (p. 422). Boredom is also expressed as a passive form of protest
against social order, in the case of many nineteenth- and twentieth-century
women (Pease, 2012), and a demonstration against
capitalistic work division and values on the part of the nineteenth-century flâneurs
(Benjamin, 2002). It may serve also as a pose of
superiority – some people pose as bored in order to demonstrate their alleged
pre-potency (Petry-Mroczkowska, 2004; Raposa, 1999),
implying that they already know everything, they have already seen all kinds of
‘wonders’ of the world and that nothing can raise their interest no longer. For
others, such as members of the leisure classes, it constitutes “a
status-maintaining device” (Klapp, 1986, p. 26), a part of the role they play in social spectacle (Veblen, 2007).
Emotions are also believed to be
specialized
modes of operation shaped by natural selection to adjust the physiological,
psychological, and behavioural parameters of the organism in ways that increase
its capacity and tendency to respond adaptively to the threats and
opportunities characteristic of specific kinds of situations (Nesse, 1990, p. 268),
and thereby to have an
evolutionary function. Boredom was beneficial for our evolutionary ancestors,
at least in a few ways: (1) it prevented energy loss on things that were
repetitive, predictable and monotonous, thus, not posing a threat (Bornstein, 1989; Heron, 1956; Todman, 2003), and thereby promoted the conservation
of energy needed to compete for scarce resources (cf. Hsee
et al., 2010, who found that people, despite
having an aversion to idleness, tend to need justification for their
busy-ness); (2) it has promoted learning, discovery and exploration ‘preventing
animals from becoming behaviourally inflexible in the face of likely environmental
changes’ (Burn, 2017; cf. Lin and Westgate, 2022), it motivates some animals to experiment with
new sources of food, play with new materials, change territories, learn new
skills – “those who had to fight boredom ended up knowing more about their
environment and having more skills than those who were satisfied with simple tasks”
(Davies and Fortney, 2012, p. 139), therefore, it has
promoted self-regulation processes, which might increase adaptability to a
changing environment (Elpidorou, 2017a). In other words, “boredom ‘punishes’ behavior lacking in meaning or optimal attentional
engagement, encouraging people to disengage from those behaviors
in the present, and making such behavior less likely
in the future” (Lin and Westgate, 2022, p. 13);
(3) it has deepened one’s perception (Lomas, 2017; Raposa, 1999) and enabled quick switches of
attention between events, increasing the chances for locating both the sources
of nourishment and danger (Mann, 2016); (4) it has been a mild form of
disgust (Miller, 1997) and analogically “[i]f disgust protects humans from infection, boredom may
protect them from ‘infectious’ social situations: those that are confined,
predictable, too samey for one’s sanity” (Toohey, 2011, p. 17;
see more in Finkielsztein, 2016).
In today’s information, entertainment, consumer,
achievement society, boredom may constitute an anaesthetic response to overload
of stimuli/information/opportunities. It is conceived as a form of adaptation
strategy to the realities of an over-stimulating environment, “a
self-protection mechanism against an overabundance of redundant stimuli” (Biceaga, 2006, p. 153), “a barrier against noise”
(Klapp, 1986, p. 9). For instance, in the
excessively stimulating urban environment, which exhausts one’s nervous system
to its extreme by enforcing the state of ‘hyper attention’ (Han, 2015), boredom is a defensive mechanism protecting one’s sanity by
distancing them from the excess of stimuli (in this capacity, it resembles the
concept of blasé coined by Georg Simmel [1950]).
The function of boredom that can also be founded in the
relevant literature is its role as a spiritus movens
of capitalism. “[B]oredom has to be incessantly
conjured in order to push people into constant action and, above all,
consumption” (Peeren, 2019, p.
105). The culture industry, as Theodor Adorno (2001)
portrayed it, or boredom industry as I would call it (Finkielsztein,
2022, in press), is an endless spiral of
passing from entertainment to boredom and backwards to the next entertainment
without sense of satisfaction. Boredom becomes “a resented and feared bugbear
of the consumer society,” because well-trained members of such a society cannot
stand “the absence or even temporary interruption of the perpetual flow of
attention-drawing, exciting novelties” (Bauman, 2007, p.
130). Nowadays, people are meticulously socialized to being addicted to novelty
and constant stimulation. In consequence, they can hardly put up with routines
and repetitions. They “experience this lack of tolerance as the uncomfortable
feeling of boredom, and it is the motivation to reduce this ennui that leads
[them] in a never-ending quest for stimulation” (Mann, 2016, p. xii).
Boredom may also serve several other functions. It acts
as a defence/protection against, or disguise for less acceptable and more
difficult emotions, such as rage, anger, anxiety, fear, concern or depression (Beckelman, 1995;
Maynard, 2002; Morrant,
1984). Evangelia Loukidou
(2008) found among nurses in a hospital ward for the
mentally ill that being bored served as a protective measure against the fear
of insanity – nurses distanced themselves from patients, did not engage with
them emotionally, and ended up feeling bored. Boredom may be a distancing
mechanism that prevents depression – when one is bored
they are simultaneously detached from the source of depression and restless to
find new meanings (Bargdill, 2014).
Experiencing boredom is also the most efficient way to
learn how to cope with the feeling. Some kinds of boredom can be beneficial for
future success; for instance, it is believed that a student’s academic boredom “prepares
its victims for the greater boredom to come” (Healy, 1984, p. 9)
in workplaces (Finkielsztein, 2013; Jablonka, 2013; Tardieu, 1913).
Boredom is a significant element of the hidden curriculum (the unofficial,
informal, implicit and often unintended rules, routines, and regulations
students learn during their education [Jackson, 1990]). As
Jack Common stated in 1951, “we learn reading and boredom, writing and boredom,
arithmetic and boredom, and so on, depending on the program, then we can, with
great certainty, take care of the most boring occupation, and we will endure it
anyway” (quoted in Meighan, 1993, pp. 80–81).
Boredom is also reflected in power relationships,
and is a tool for controlling society. The power to enforce boredom in
others epitomises the higher social position – teachers subject their students
to boredom during classes, doctors impose boredom on patients who have to wait
for an appointment, courts sentenced offenders to the punishment of boredom
(prison), parents punish their children with various kinds of limitations and
constraints of freedom of action (e.g., grounding), governments set a curfew limiting
a citizen’s opportunities for leisure activities or terrorise society, so
people are afraid to leave their homes.
Boredom also allows people to perceive things as
interesting – if boredom did not exist, everything would appear
indistinguishable, bearing no particular meaning for
individuals. The comparison with something conceived as boring enables people
to find meaning and differentiate what interests them (Kolakowski,
1999).
6. Positive Outcomes
Apart from serving many
vital functions, boredom may result in outcomes that are generally perceived as
positive. One such product of this feeling may be reflection and self-cognition
(Bizior-Dombrowska, 2016; Brodsky,
1995; Darden and Marks, 1999; Gehring, 1997).
Boredom gives people a chance to be contemplative, to “develop a critical
awareness of those activities which are ordinarily too banal or repetitive to
merit attention” (Moran, 2003, p. 75). Boredom also constitutes
an encounter with oneself, and is believed to enhance the identity forging
process (Cioran, 1995; de
Chateaubriand, 2010; Johnsen, 2011; Markowski, 1999), and
this is why it is suggested to be of essential significance for children
(Phillips, 1993) – in boredom one is able to
reflect on oneself, get to know what they like/dislike, who they are, and to
which end/future/goal they aspire.
At times, boredom is also associated with creativity
(stimulus independent thoughts [Takeuchi et al., 2012]), or counter-factual imagination (Chylińska, 2017; see
also Brisset and Snow, 1993; Gabelman, 2010; Gasper and Middlewood,
2014; Sandywell,
2016; Toohey, 2011).
Boredom is perceived as “the pathway to enlightenment” (Keen, quoted in Brisset and Snow, 1993, p.
243), “the mystical feeling which drives the philosopher from abstract thinking
to intuition” (Marx, 1976, p. 398), “the mother of all
invention,” “the mother of the Muses” (Goethe, quoted in Kuhn, 1976, p. 184), and “a critical resource that pushes us to seek the
unfamiliar” (de Vries, 2015, p. 170). Some authors claim that
boredom is associated with an extensive type of attention (scattered,
non-focused on one thing [Kolańczyk, 2011]), which enables creative processes by
enhancing remote associations (Chylińska, 2016), and as the state that can activate the
default mode network (Raichle et al., 2001; Zomorodi, 2017), in which the brain is not occupied by
external stimuli and remains active, which is also claimed to be beneficial for
creativity. All these things are believed to contribute to serendipity – the
accidental discovery of an important solution by a theoretically prepared mind
(Merton, 1968), an example of which may be
Archimedes’ ‘eureka’ moment. Boredom was found to increase creativity in tasks
requiring serial responses – in the research of Daniel Schubert (1977) participants who thought about solving a task
for 60 minutes invented more solutions that were more creative than those who
worked in three 20-minute sets separated by 20-minute breaks. Those who worked
continuously after 20 minutes became bored with their earlier answers and
invented new ones, while those who worked in batches after each break returned
to what they had finished, reworking their previous answers. Sandi Mann and
Rebekah Cadman (2014) suggested that being boredom can
lead to enhanced creativity in terms of quantity, but not quality – bored
participants listed more uses for two polystyrene cups than participants in a
control group, yet the answers were not significantly more creative. In total,
the connection between boredom and creativity is far from being proved, yet, it is suggested that within some limits boredom at
times might boost some individuals’ creativity (primarily those who are already
creative, Ros Velasco, 2022).
As American anthropologist Ralph Linton (1936) suggested, “[i]t seems probable that the
human capacity for being bored, rather than man’s social or natural needs, lies
at the root of man’s cultural advance” (p. 90; cf. Nisbet, 1983). Indeed, data derived from both scientific literature and
auto-biographical material suggests that many of the most active culture
producers (artists) experienced boredom and were even motivated by it to write,
paint or compose (Spacks, 1995). Artists admitted to create out of
boredom include, among others, French writers George Sand (Bizior-Dombrowska,
2016), Voltaire, Alfred de Musset and Stendhal
(Kuhn, 1976); Polish writer Witold Gombrowicz (Sienkiewicz, 1999);
Italian writer Alberto Moravia (Ejder, 2005); the English poet George Byron (Gabelman, 2010); the English scholar and author of The Anatomy of Melancholy, Robert Burton
(2009); the French painter, Eugène Delacroix (Śniedziewski, 2011); many
avant-garde artists (e.g. Dadaists [Haladyn, 2015; Jawłowska, 1975]), and
comics writers (Schneider, 2012). Boredom was, in its metaphysical,
existential sense, conceived by many artists as an indispensable and inevitable
part of all creative work. Friedrich Nietzsche even stated that “[f]or the
thinker and for all inventive spirits, boredom is that disagreeable “lull” of
the soul that precedes a happy voyage and cheerful winds; he has to endure it,
must await its effect on him” (2001, p. 57).
In that sense, boredom is a philosophical phenomenon par excellence – it may be
even suggested that philosophy was founded out of boredom. It constitutes a
powerful tool enabling the human being to leave the Platonian cave, i.e., the
world of illusions, imitations, falsity and inauthenticity; it is ‘the moment
of vision,’ in which we are able to see the truth of our existence, our Dasein (‘being-in-the-world’ [Heidegger,
1995]). In boredom, when one is not occupied by
anything or oneself, one encounters/becomes aware of nihility and acquires more
accurate perspective on human life. One realises that the existence is
meaningless and finite – non-existence (death) becomes obvious as an
alternative to Dasein.
7. Conclusions
As proven by extensive
literature, boredom is far from being insignificant, minor affective state. It
has been found prevalent irrespectively of age, gender, culture or social
class. Boredom affects all main spheres of human life – work, leisure, education,
romantic relationships, sex life, even religious life. It has also been
evidenced that boredom has many significant consequences. It has been
associated with, among others, risk-taking behaviours, overeating, impulse shopping, (self-)destructive and violent behaviours
including criminal and delinquent activities, relational problems and divorces.
Boredom has also been found to be a corelative of many negative affective
states. Yet, boredom may serve numerous significant functions as well. As an
emotion, it is important for cognition, motivation and communication. In that
capacity, boredom is indicated to form a motivating/energising force, a
catalyst for action that ‘pushes’ one to seek for activity that seems
meaningful or interesting, to engage in challenge-seeking behaviour. It has
also had evolutionary meaning for human beings. In today’s
society, it serves as a defensive mechanism against overload of stimuli,
but somehow to the contrary is also found to be a basic mechanism animating
current consumerism. Boredom is also conceived to be a catalyst for reflection,
self-cognition, creativity, and as a consequence a
rudimental element of culture production and its advances. All arguments
gathered in this article was meant to evidence the significance of boredom and
prove that it is a worth researching phenomenon. If, as material gathered here
seems to suggest, boredom is an inevitable part of human life and condition,
and “an inescapable fact like the illness that comes in its time” (Tardieu, 1913, p. 233), we, as researchers,
should pay more attention to it, to enhance positive outcomes and limited
negative consequences of that emotional state.
References
Abramson,
E., and Stinson., S. (1977). Boredom and Eating in Obese and Non-Obese
Individuals. Addictive Behaviours, 2(4), 181–185.
Adorno, T. (2001). Free Time. In The Culture Industry. Selected
Essays on Mass Culture (pp. 187–197). Routledge.
Adorno, T. (2006). Minima Moralia:
Reflections on a Damaged Life. Verso.
Aho,
K. (2007). Simmel on Acceleration, Boredom, and Extreme Aesthesia. Journal
for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 37(4), 447–462. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5914.2007.00345.x
Anderson,
B. (2015). Boredom, Excitement and Other Security Affects. Dialogues in
Human Geography, 5(3), 271–274. https://doi.org/10.1177/2043820615607759
Asimov, I. (1964). Visit to the
World’s Fair of 2014. The New York Times. Retrieved from https://archive.nytimes.com/www.nytimes.com/books/97/03/23/lifetimes/asi-v-fair.html
Avramenko, R. (2004). Bedeviled by Boredom: A
Voegelinian Reading of Dostoevsky’s Possessed. Humanitas,
17(1–2), 108–139.
Baker,
P. (1992). Bored and Busy: Sociology of Knowledge of Clerical Workers. Sociological Perspectives, 35(3),
489–503. https://doi.org/10.2307/1389331
Baker,
R., D’Mello, S., Rodrigo, M. M., and Graesser, A. (2010).
Better to Be Frustrated than Bored: The Incidence,
Persistence, and Impact of Learners’ Cognitive-Affective States During Interactions
with Three Different Computer-Based Learning Environments. International
Journal of Human Computer Studies, 68(4), 223–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2009.12.003
Barbalet, J. (1999). Boredom and Social Meaning. The British Journal of
Sociology, 50(4), 631–646. https://doi.org/10.1080/000713199358572
Bargdill, R. (2014). Toward a Theory of Habitual Boredom. Janus Head, 13(2),
93–111.
Bartone, P. (2005). The Need for Positive Meaning in Military Operations:
Reflections on Abu Ghraib. Military Psychology, 17(4), 315–324. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327876mp1704_5
Bauman,
Z. (2007). Consuming Life. Polity Press.
Beckelman, L. (1995). Boredom. Crestwood House.
Belton, T., and Priyadharshini, E. (2007). Boredom and Schooling: A Cross-Disciplinary
Exploration. Cambridge Journal of Education, 37(4), 579–595. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057640701706227
Bench,
S., and Lench, H. (2013). On the Function of Boredom. Behavioral
Sciences, 3(3), 459–472. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs3030459
Bengtsson,
T. (2012). Boredom and Action--Experiences from Youth Confinement. Journal
of Contemporary Ethnography, 41(5), 526–553. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891241612449356
Benjamin,
W. (2002). The Arcades Project. The Belknap Press of Harvard University
Press.
Bergler, E. (1945). On the Disease-Entity Boredom (“Alysosis”)
and Its Psychopathology. Psychiatric Quarterly, 19(1), 38–51.
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, Arousal and Curiosity. McGraw-Hill.
Bernstein,
H. (1975). Boredom and the Ready-Made Life. Social Research, 42(3),
512–537.
Biceaga, V. (2006). Temporality and Boredom. Continental Philosophy Review,
39(2), 135–153. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11007-006-9015-4
Binnema, D. (2004). Interrelations of Psychiatric Patient Experiences of
Boredom and Mental Health. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 25(8),
833–842. https://doi.org/10.1080/01612840490506400
Bizior-Dombrowska,
M. (2016). Romantyczna nuda. Wielka nostalgia za
niczym [Romantic Boredom.
Great Nostalgia for Nothing].
Wydawnictwo Naukowe Uniwersytetu Mikołaja Kopernika.
Blaszczynski, A., McConaghy, N., and Frankova,
A. (1990). Boredom Proneness in Pathological
Gambling. Psychological Reports, 67(1), 35–42. https://doi.org/10.2466/PR0.67.5.35-42
Bornstein,
R. (1989). Exposure and Affect: Overview and Metaanalysis of Research, 1968-1987. Psychological
Bulletin, 102, 265–289.
Bracke, P., Bruynooghe, K., and Verhaeghe, M.
(2006). Boredom During Day Activity Programs in Rehabilitation Centers. Sociological Perspectives, 49(2),
191–215. https://doi.org/10.1525/sop.2006.49.2.191
Bracke, P., and Verhaeghe, M. (2010).
Structural Determinants of Boredom Among the Clients of Psychosocial and Vocational
Rehabilitation Centers. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 40(8), 1969–1998. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00647.x
Branković, S. (2015). Boredom, Dopamin, and the Thrill
of Psychosis: Psychiatry in a New Key. Psychiatria
Danubina, 27(2), 126–137.
Brisset, D., and Snow, R. (1993). Boredom: Where the Future Isn’t. Symbolic
Interaction, 16(3), 237–256.
Britton, A., and Shipley, M. J. (2010). Bored to Death? International
Journal of Epidemiology, 39(2), 370–371. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyp404
Burn,
C. (2017). Bestial Boredom: A Biological Perspective on Animal Boredom and Suggestions
for Its Scientific Investigation. Animal Behaviour, 130, 141–151.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.06.006
Burton, R. (2009). The Anatomy of Melancholy.
Ex-classics Project.
Byron, G. (2006) Don Juan. In Selected Poems
of Lord Byron (pp. 51–556). Wordsworth Poetry Library.
Calhoun,
C. (2011). Living with Boredom. Sophia, 50(2), 269–279. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11841-011-0239-3
Campagne, D. (2012). When Therapists Run out of Steam: Professional Boredom or Burnout? Revista de Psicopatología y Psicología Clínica, 17(1), 75–85.
Carroll, B., Parker, P., and Inkson, K. (2010). Evasion of Boredom: An Unexpected Spur to Leadership? Human Relations,
63(7), 1031–1049. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726709349864
Chaney, M., and Blalock, A. (2006). Boredom Proneness, Social
Connectedness, and Sexual Addiction Among Men Who Have Sex with Male Internet
Users. Journal of Addictions & Offender Counseling, 26(2), 111–122. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1874.2006.tb00012.x
Chin,
A., Markey, A., Bhargava, S., Kassam, K., and Loewenstein, G. (2017). Emotion
Bored in the USA: Experience Sampling and Boredom in Everyday Life. Emotion, 17(2), 359–368. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000232
Chylińska, M. (2016). O dwóch pojęciach
nudy w kontekście rozważań nad tym,
czy nuda jest twórcza [On Two
Notions of Boredom in the Context of Considering Whether Boredom Is Creative]. Stan Rzeczy, 11(2), 139–148.
Chylińska, M. (2017). Counterfactual
Imagination as a Mental Tool for Innovation. AVANT, 8, 241–251.
Cioran,
E. (1995). Tears and Saints. The Univeristy of
Chicago Press.
Clemons,
S. (2013, August 20). We Were Bored... So We Decided to Kill Somebody. The
Atlantic. Retrieved from https://www.theatlantic.com/national/archive/2013/08/we-were-bored-so-we-decided-to-kill-somebody/278858/
Conrad,
P. (1997). It’s Boring: Notes on the Meanings of Boredom in Everyday Life. Qualitative
Sociology as Everyday Life, 20(4), 123–133. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024747820595
Corvinelli, A. (2007). An Emerging Theory of Boredom in Recovery for Adult
Substance Users with HIV/AIDS Attending an Urban Day Treatment Program. Occupational
Therapy in Mental Health, 23(2), 27–50. https://doi.org/10.1300/J004v23n02_02
Crangle,
S. (2008). The Time Being: On Woolf and Boredom. MFS Modern Fiction Studies,
54(2), 209–229. https://doi.org/10.1353/mfs.0.0016
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond Boredom and Anxiety.
Jossey-Bass Publishers.
Czocher, A. (2018). Okupacyjna nuda w egodokumentach. Przykład Krakowa z lat 1939‒1945 [Occupational
Boredom in Egodocuments.
The Example of Krakow from
1939-1945]. Maska, 37(1), 129–140.
Dahlen, E., Martin, R., Ragan, K., and Kuhlman, M. (2004). Boredom
Proneness in Anger and Aggression: Effects of Impulsiveness and Sensation
Seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1615–1627. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.016
Dahlen, E., Martin, R., Ragan, K., and Kuhlman, M. (2005). Driving Anger,
Sensation Seeking, Impulsiveness, and Boredom Proneness in the Prediction of
Unsafe Driving. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 37(2), 341–348.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2004.10.006
Davies,
H. (1926). Discussion on the Physical and Mental Effects of Fatigue in Modern
Industry. The British Medical Journal, 2(3427), 472–479. https://doi.org/10.2307/25325687
Davies, J., and Fortney, M. (2012). The Menton Theory of Engagement and Boredom. First
Annual Conference on Advances in Cognitive Systems, 131–143.
de Chateaubriand, F.-R. (2010). René. Retrieved from https://www.poetryintranslation.com/PITBR/Chateaubriand/ChateaubriandRene.php
de Chenne, T. (1998). Boredom as a Clinical Issue. Psychotherapy: Theory,
Research, Practice, Training, 25(1), 71–81. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0085325
Development Dimensions International (2004).
Faking It: An Examination of the Impacto of Values and Culture on Employee
Attitudes to Work. Coach Matching. Retrieved from www.coachmatching.com/library/item/download/11_3899f2c4b0ffd03ef7f31ac6c7b39194
de Vries, M. F. R. K. (2015). Doing Nothing and Nothing to Do: The Hidden Value of Empty Time and Boredom.
Organizational Dynamics, 44(3), 169–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.orgdyn.2015.05.002
Drory,
A. (1982). Individual Differences in Boredom Proneness and Task Effectiveness
at Work. Personnel Psychology, 35(1), 141–152. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1982.tb02190.x
Ejder,
Ö. (2005). Spaces of Boredom: Imagination and the Ambivalence of Limits.
Bilkent University.
Elpidorou, A. (2015). The Quiet Alarm. Life Without Boredom Would Be a Nightmare.
Aeon. Retrieved from https://aeon.co/essays/life-without-boredom-would-be-a-nightmare
Elpidorou, A. (2017a). The Bored Mind Is a Guiding Mind: Toward a Regulatory Theory
of Boredom. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 17(3),
455–484. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-017-9515-1
Elpidorou, A. (2017b). The Moral Dimensions of Boredom: A Call for Research. Review
of General Psychology, 21(1), 30–48. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000098
Esman,
A. (1979). Some Reflections on Boredom. Journal of the American
Psychoanalytic Association, 27(2), 423–439.
Fahlman,
S., Mercer-Lynn, K., Flora, D., and Eastwood, J. (2013). Development and
Validation of the Multidimensional State Boredom Scale. Assessment, 20(1),
68–85. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111421303
Fahlman,
S., Mercer, K., Gaskovski, P., Eastwood, A., and Eastwood, J. (2009). Does a Lack of Life Meaning
Cause Boredom? Results from Psychometric, Longitudinal, and Experimental
Analyses. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28(3),
307–340. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2009.28.3.307
Farmer, R., and Sundberg, N. (1986). Boredom Proneness - The Development
and Correlates of a New Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50(1),
4–17. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5001
Farnworth,
L. (1998). Doing, Being, and Boredom. Journal of Occupational Science, 5(3),
140–146. https://doi.org/10.1080/14427591.1998.9686442
Ferrell,
J. (2004). Boredom, Crime and Criminology. Theoretical Criminology, 8(3),
287–302. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362480604044610
Finkielsztein, M. (2013). Nuda Na Zajęciach Uniwersyteckich. Percepcja Nudy Wśród
Studentów Uniwersytetu Warszawskiego [Boredom During University Classes. The Perception of Boredom Among Students of the University of Warsaw]. MA Thesis, The University of Warsaw.
Finkielsztein, M. (2016). Nuda a Wstręt. Przesyt, Ennui i Wstręt Do Życia [Boredom and Disgust. Satiety, Ennui and Disugst to Life]. Stan Rzeczy, 11(2), 61–73.
Finkielsztein, M. (2021). Boredom and Academic Work. Routledge.
Finkielsztein, M. (2022). Consumer Boredom: Boredom as a Subliminal Mood of Consumer
Capitalism. European Journal of American Studies. [In Press]
Frederiksen,
M. (2017). Joyful Pessimism: Marginality, Disengagement, and the Doing of Nothing.
Focaal, 78, 9–22. https://doi.org/10.3167/fcl.2017.780102
Fromm, E. (1973). The Anatomy of
Human Destructiveness. Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
Fromm, E. (2011). The Pathology of Normalcy.
Lantern Books.
Fuller,
C. (2011). Timepass and Boredom in Modern India. Anthropology of This
Century. Retrieved from http://eprints.lse.ac.uk/38021/
Gabelman, D. (2010). Bubbles, Butterflies and Bores: Play and Boredom in Don
Juan. The Byron Journal, 38(2), 145–156. https://doi.org/10.3828/bj.2010.23
Gamsby,
P. (2012). The Black Sun of Boredom: Henri Lefebvre and the Critique of
Everyday Life. Laurentian University.
Gardiner,
M. (2012). Henri Lefebvre and the “Sociology of Boredom.” Theory, Culture
& Society, 29(2), 37–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/0263276411417460
Gardiner, M., and Haladyn, J. J. (Eds.). (2016). Boredom Studies Reader. Frameworks and
perspectives. Routledge.
Gasper, K., and Middlewood, B. (2014). Approaching Novel Thoughts: Understanding Why Elation and Boredom
Promote Associative Thought More Than Distress and Relaxation. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 50–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.12.007
Gehring,
V. (1997). Tedium Vitae: Or, My Life as a “Net Serf.” Ratio, 10(2), 124–140.
Goetz,
T., Frenzel, A., Hall, N., Nett, U., Pekrun, R., and Lipnevich, A. (2014). Types of Boredom: An Experience Sampling
Approach. Motivation and Emotion, 38(3), 401–419. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9385-y
Goldberg,
Y., Eastwood, J., Laguardia, J., and Danckert, J. (2011). Boredom: An
Emotional Experience Distinct from Apathy, Anhedonia, or Depression. Journal
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30(6), 647–666. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2011.30.6.647
Grazian, D. (2003). Blue Chicago: The
Search for Authenticity. In Urban Blues Clubs. The Univeristy
of Chicago Press.
Haga,
S. (1984). An Experimental Study of Signal Vigilance Errors in Train Driving. Ergonomics,
27(7), 755–765. https://doi.org/10.1002/2015GC005973.Received
Haladyn, J. (2015). Boredom and Art: Passions of the Will to Boredom. Zero
Books.
Haller,
M., Hadler, M., and Kaup, G. (2013). Leisure Time in
Modern Societies: A New Source of Boredom and Stress? Social Indicators
Research, 111(2), 403–434. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-012-0023-y
Han, H. Ch. (2015). The Burnout
Society. Stanford University Press.
Hanby, M. (2004). The Culture of Death,
the Ontology of Boredom, and the Resistance of Joy. Communio. International
Catholic Review, 31(2), 181–199.
Hancock, P. (2017). On Bored to
Mars. The Journal of Astrosociology, 2, 103–120.
Hancock, P., and Krueger, G. (2010). Hours of Boredom - Moments of
Terror. Temporal Desynchrony in Military and Security Force Operations. Center for Technology and National Security Policy: Defense
and Technology Paper.
Harasymchuk, C., and Fehr, B. (2010). A Script
Analysis of Relational Boredom: Causes, Feelings, and Coping Strategies. Journal
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29(9), 988–1019.
Harasymchuk, C., and Fehr, B. (2012).
Development of a Prototype-Based Measure of Relational Boredom. Personal Relationships,
19(1), 162–181. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01346.x
Harasymchuk, C., and Fehr, B. (2013). A
Prototype Analysis of Relational Boredom. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 30(5), 627–646. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407512464483
Harper,
T. (1987). The Best Brightest, Bored and Burned Out. ABA Journal, 73(7),
28–31.
Havermans, R., Vancleef, L., Kalamatianos,
A., and Nederkoorn, C. (2015). Eating and Inflicting Pain
out of Boredom. Appetite, 85, 52–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.11.007
Healy,
S. D. (1984). Boredom, Self, and Culture. Fairleigh Dickenson University
Press.
Helvétius, C. A. (1810). De L’Esprit; or, Essays on the Mind
and Its Several Faculties. Albion Press.
Heron,
W. (1956). The Pathology of Boredom. Scientific American, 196(1),
52–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0046-8177(78)80028-8
Hill,
A. B. (1975). Work Variety and Individual Differences in Occupational Boredom. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 60(1), 128–131. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076346
Hill, A. B., and Perkins, R. E. (1985). Towards a Model of Boredom. British
Journal of Psychology, 76, 235–240. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1985.tb01947.x
Hoeyberghs, L., Verté, E., Verté,
D., Schols, J., and de Witte, N. (2018).
Hopelessness, Life Dissatisfaction and Boredom Among Older People. British
Journal of Community Nursing, 23(8), 400–405.
Homel, R., Tomsen,
S., and Thommeny, J. (1992). Public Drinking and
Violence: Not Just an Alcohol Problem. Journal of Drug Issues, 22(3),
679–697.
Hsee, C., Yang, A., and Wang, L. (2010). Idleness Aversion and the Need for Justifiable
Busyness. Psychological Science, 21(7), 926–930. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610374738
Inge,
W. (1940). Escape. In The Fall of the Idols (pp. 386–399). Putnam.
Irvine,
I. (2001). The Angel of Luxury and Sadness. Vol. 1: The Emergence of the
Normative Ennui Cycle. BookSurge
Publishing.
Iso-Ahola, S., and Weissinger,
E. (1990). Perceptions of Boredom in Leisure: Conceptualization, Reliability
and Validity of the Leisure Boredom Scale. Journal of Leisure Research, 22(1),
1–17.
Iso-Ahola, S., and Crowley, E. (1991).
Adolescent Substance Abuse and Leisure Boredom. Journal of Leisure Research,
23(3), 260–271.
Jablonka, E. (2013). Social Dimensions of Boredom in Classrooms from Germany,
Hong Kong and the United States. Second Manchester Conference on Mathematics
Education and Contemporary Theory, United Kingdom.
Jackson,
P. (1990). Life in Classrooms. Teachers College Press.
Jahoda,
M., Lazarsfeld, P., and Zeisel, H. (2009). Marienthal: The Sociography of an Unemployed
Community. Transaction Publishers.
Jawłowska, A. (1975). Drogi
Kontrkultury [Roads of Counterculture]. Państwowy Instytut Wydawniczy.
Johansson,
G. (1989). Job Demands and Stress Reactions in Repetitive and Uneventful Monotony
at Work. International Journal of Health Services, 19(2),
365–377. https://doi.org/10.2190/XYP9-VK4Y-9H80-VV3K
Johnsen,
R. (2011). On Boredom: A Note on Experience Without Qualities. Ephemera,
11(4), 482–489.
Johnsen,
R. (2016). Boredom and Organization Studies. Organization Studies, 37(10),
1403–1415. https://doi.org/10.1177/0170840616640849
ISSP Research Group (2009). International
Social Survey Programme: Leisure Time and Sports - ISSP 2007. GESIS Data
Archive, Cologne. ZA4850 Data file Version 2.0.0, https://doi.org/10.4232/1.10079
Kass, S., Beede, K., and Vodanovich, S. (2010).
Self-Report Measures of Distractibility as Correlates of Simulated Driving Performance.
Accident Analysis and Prevention, 42(3), 874–880. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2009.04.012
Kass, S., Vodanovich, S., and Callender, A. (2001). State-Trait Boredom:
Relationship to Absenteeism, Tenure, and Job Satisfaction. Journal of
Business and Psychology, 16(2), 317–327. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011121503118
Kerce,
E. (1985). Boredom at Work: Implications for the Design of Jobs with Variable
Requirements. Navy Personnel Research and Development Center.
Kierkegaard,
S. (1843). Either/Or. Retrieved from http://sqapo.com/CompleteText-Kierkegaard-EitherOr.htm
Klapp,
O. (1986). Overload and Boredom. Essays on the Quality of Life in the
Information Society. Greenwood Press.
Koball, A., Meers, M., Storfer-Isser,
A., Domoff, S., and Musher-Eizenman,
D. (2012). Eating When Bored: Revision of the Emotional Eating Scale with a Focus
on Boredom. Health Psychology, 31(4), 521–524. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025893
Kolakowski, L. (1999). On Boredom. In Freedom, Fame, Lying and Betrayal: Essays
on Everyday Life (pp. 85–94). Penguin Books.
Kolańczyk, A. (2011). Uwaga ekstensywna.
Model ekstensywności vs. intensywności
uwagi [Extensive Attention. Model of Extensiveness Attention vs. Intensiveness Attention]. Studia
Psychologiczne, 49(3), 7–27. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10167-010-0024-x
Korczak, J. (2003). Ghetto Diary.
Yale University Press.
Krotava, I., and Todman,
M. (2014). Boredom Severity, Depression and
Alcohol Consumption in Belarus. Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Science, 2(1), 73–83.
Kuhn,
R. (1976). The Demon of Noontide. Ennui in Western Literature. Princeton
University Press.
Kustermans, J., and Ringmar, E.
(2011). Modernity, Boredom, and War: A Suggestive Essay. Review of
International Studies, 37(4), 1775–1792. https://doi.org/10.2307/23025575
Langan-Fox, J., Sankey, M., and Canty, J. (2009).
Human Factors Measurement for Future Air Traffic Control Systems. The
Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 51, 595–637.
Larson, R., and Richards, M. (1991). Boredom in the Middle School Years:
Blaming Schools versus Blaming Students. American Journal of Education, 99(4),
418–443. https://doi.org/10.1086/443992
Laugesen, A. (2012). Boredom is the Enemy. The Intellectual and Imaginative
Lives of Australian Soldiers in the Great War and Beyond. Routledge.
Lee, T. W. (1986). Toward the Development and Calidation of a Measure of Job Boredom. Manhattan
College Journal of Business, 15, 22–28.
Lepenies, W. (1992). Melancholy and Society. Harvard University Press.
LePera,
N. (2011). Relationships Between Boredom Proneness, Mindfulness, Anxiety, Depression,
and Substance Use. The New School Psychology Bulletin, 8(2),
15–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/e741452011-003
Lin, Y., and Westgate, E. (2022). The Origins
of Boredom. In The Oxford Handbook of Evolution and the Emotions. Oxford
University Press. [In Press]
Linton, R. (1936). The Study of Man: An
Introduction. Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Lomas,
T. (2017). A Meditation on Boredom: Re-Appraising Its Value Through Introspective
Phenomenology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 14(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/14780887.2016.1205695
Loukidou, E. (2008). Boredom in the Workplace: A Qualitative Study of Psychiatric
Nurses in Greece. PhD Thesis, Loughborough University.
Lyng,
S. (Ed.). (2005). Edgework. The Sociology of Risk-Taking. Routledge.
Macklem, G. (2015). Boredom in the Classroom. Addressing Student Motivation,
Self-Regulation, and Engagement in Learning. Springer.
Maeland, B., and Brunstad,
P. O. (2009). Enduring Military Boredom. From 1750
to the Present. Palgrave Macmillan.
Mains,
D. (2007). Neoliberal Times: Progress, Boredom, and Shame Among Young Men in
Urban Ethiopia. American Ethnologist, 34(4), 659–673. https://doi.org/10.1525/ae.2007.34.4.659.American
Malachowski, D. (2005). Wasted Time at Work
Costing Companies Billions. Salary.com. Retrieved from https://apexassisting.com/wasted-time-at-work-costing-companies-billions/
Malamud,
R. (2016, July 14). One Big Yawn? The Academics Bewitched by Boredom. Times
Higher Education. Retrieved from https://www.timeshighereducation.com/features/one-big-yawn-the-academics-bewitched-by-boredom
Malkovsky, E., Merrifield, C., Goldberg, Y., and Danckert,
J. (2012). Exploring the Relationship Between Boredom and Sustained Attention. Experimental
Brain Research, 221(1), 59–67. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3147-z
Mallory,
A. (2003). Burke, Boredom, and the Theater of Counterrevolution. PMLA, 118(2),
224–238.
Mann,
S. (2007). The Boredom Boom. The Psychologist, 20(2), 2–5.
Mann,
S. (2016). The Upside of Downtime. Why Boredom is Good. Robinson.
Mann,
S., and Cadman, R. (2014). Does Being Bored Make Us More Creative? Creativity
Research Journal, 26(2), 165–173. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2014.901073
Markowski, M. P. (1999). L’Ennui. Ułamek historii [L’Ennui. A Piece of History]. In Nuda w kulturze
[Boredom in Culture] (pp. 290–316). Rebis.
Marshall,
C, Roy, L., Becker, A., Nguyen, M., Barbic, S., Tjornstrand,
C., Gewurtz, R., and Wickett,
S. (2019). Boredom and Homelessness: A Scoping Review. Journal of
Occupational Science, 27(1), 107–124.
Martin,
M., Sadlo, G., and Stew, G. (2006). The Phenomenon of Boredom. Qualitative
Research in Psychology, 3, 193–211. https://doi.org/10.1139/b87-051
Martin, P. (2009). Sex, Drugs and
Chocolate: The Science of Pleasure. Harper Collins.
Marx, K. (1976). Capital: Volume
1. Penguin.
Masquelier,
A. (2013). Teatime: Boredom and the Temporalities of Young Men in Niger. Africa:
Journal Fo the International African Institute, 83(3), 470–491. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0001972013000272
Masquelier,
A. (2019). Fada: Boredom and Belonging in Niger. The University of
Chicago Press.
Meighan, R. (1993). Socjologia Edukacji
[A Sociology of Educating]. Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Mikołaja Kopernika.
Mercer-Lynn, K., Flora, D., Fahlman, S., and Eastwood, J.
(2011). The Measurement of Boredom: Differences Between Existing Self-Report
Scales. Assessment, 20(5), 585–596. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111408229
Mercer-Lynn, K., Hunter, J., and Eastwood, J. (2013). Is Trait Boredom
Redundant? Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 32(8),
897–916. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2013.32.8.897
Mercer, K., and Eastwood, J. (2010). Is Boredom Associated with Problem
Gambling Behaviour? It Depends on What You Mean by ‘Boredom.’ International
Gambling Studies, 10(1), 91–104. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459791003754414
Merrifield, C., and Danckert, J. (2014). Characterizing the Psychophysiological
Signature of Boredom. Experimental Brain Research, 232(2),
481–491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3755-2
Merton, R. (1968). Social Theory
and Social Structure. The Free Press.
Miller,
W. I. (1997). The Anatomy of Disgust. Harvard University Press.
Misek,
R. (2010). Dead Time: Cinema, Heidegger, and Boredom. Continuum, 24(5),
777–785. https://doi.org/10.1080/10304312.2010.505331
Misztal, B. (2016). The Ambiguity of Everyday Experience: Between Normality and
Boredom. Qualitative Sociology Review, 12(4), 100–119.
Moran,
J. (2003). Benjamin and Boredom. Critical Quaterly, 45, 168–181.
Moravia, A. (1965). The Empty
Canvas. Penguin Books.
Morrant, J. C. A. (1984). Boredom in Psychiatric Practice. Canadian Journal
of Psychiatry, 29(5), 431–434.
Moynihan,
A., van Tilburg, W., Igou, E., Wisman, A., Donnelly, A., and Mulcaire, J.
(2015). Eaten up by Boredom: Consuming Food to Escape Awareness of the Bored
Self. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 369. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00369
Musharbash, Y. (2007). Boredom, Time, and Modernity: An Example from Aboriginal
Australia. American Anthropologist, 109(2), 307–317. https://doi.org/10.1525/AA.2007.109.2.307.308
Nederkoorn, C., Vancleef, L., Wilkenhöner,
A., Claes, L., and Havermans, R. (2016). Self-Inflicted
Pain out of Boredom. Psychiatry Research, 237, 127–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.063
Nesse,
R. (1990). Evolutionary Explanations of Emotions. Human Nature, 1(3),
261–289. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02733986
Nett,
U., Goetz, T., and Hall, N. (2011). Coping with Boredom in School: An Experience
Sampling Perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36(1),
49–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.003
Newberry, A., and Duncan, R. (2001). Roles of Boredom and Life Goals in
Juvenile Delinquency. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31,
527–541. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb02054.x
Newell,
S., Harries, P., and Ayers, S. (2012). Boredom Proneness in a Psychiatric Inpatient
Population. The International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 58(5),
488–495. https://doi.org/10.1177/0020764011408655
Newport, F. (Ed.). (2013). The
Gallup Poll. Public Opinion 2013. Rowman & Littlefield.
Ng,
A., Liu, Y., Chen, J., and Eastwood, J. (2015). Culture and State Boredom: A Comparison
Between European Canadians and Chinese. Personality and Individual
Differences, 75, 13–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.052
Ngai,
S. (2005). Ugly Feelings. Harvard University Press.
Nietzsche, F. (2001). The Gay
Science. Cambridge University Press.
Nisbet,
R. (1983). Boredom. In Prejudices: A Philosophical Dictionary. Harvard
University Press.
O’Hanlon,
J. (1981). Boredom: Practical Consequences and a Theory. Acta Psychologica, 49(1), 53–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(81)90033-0
O’Neill,
B. (2014). Cast Aside: Boredom, Downward Mobility, and Homelessness in
Post-Communist Bucharest. Cultural Anthropology, 29(1), 8–31. https://doi.org/10.14506/ca29.1.03
Parasuraman, S., and Purohit, Y. (2000). Distress and Boredom Among
Orchestra Musicians: The Two Faces of Stress. Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology, 5(1), 74–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.74
Pascal,
B. (1910). Thoughts, Letters and Minor Works. P. F. Collier & Son
Corporation.
Patterson, I., Pegg, S., and Dobson-Patterson, R. (2000).
Exploring the Links between Leisure Boredom and Alcohol Use among Youth in
Rural and Urban Areas of Australia. Journal of Park and Recreation
Administration, 18(3), 53–75.
Paulsen,
R. (2015). Empty Labor: Indleness and Workplace Resistance. Cambridge University Press.
Pease,
A. (2012). Modernism, Feminism and the Culture of Boredom. Cambridge
University Press.
Peeren,
E. (2019). You Must (Not) Be Bored! Boredom and Creativity in Global
Capitalism. In Boredom, Shanzhai, and Digitisation in the TIme of
Creative China (pp. 101–109). Amsterdam University Press.
Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L., Stupnisky, R., and
Perry, R. (2010). Boredom in Achievement Settings: Exploring Control–Value Antecedents
and Performance Outcomes of a Neglected Emotion. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 102(3), 531–549. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019243
Petry-Mroczkowska,
J. (2004). Acedia – lenistwo [Acedia – Laziness].
In Siedem grzechów głównych dzisiaj [Seven Deadly Sins] (pp. 177–207). Znak.
Phillips,
S. (2016). Police Discretion and Boredom: What Officers Do When There Is
Nothing to Do. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 45(5),
580–601. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891241615587385
Raichle, M., Snyderm
A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G.
L. (2001). A Default Mode of Brain Function. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 98(2), 676–682.
Raposa,
M. (1985). Boredom and the Religious Imagination. Journal of the American
Academy of Religion, 53(1), 75–91.
Raposa,
M. (1999). Boredom and the Religious Imagination. University of Virginia
Press.
Reid, R., Garos, S., and Carpenter, B. (2011).
Reliability, Validity, and Psychometric Development of the Hypersexual Behavior Inventory in an Outpatient Sample of Men. Sexual
Addiction and Compulsivity, 18(1), 30–51.
Rhodes,
E. (2015). The Exciting Side of Boredom. Psychologist, 28(4),
278–281.
Rhym,
J. (2012). Towards a Phenomenology of Cinematic Mood: Boredom and the Affect of Time in Antonioni’s L’eclisse.
New Literary History, 43, 477–501.
Ros Velasco, J. (2017). Boredom: A Comprehensive Study of the State of Affairs. THÉMATA. Revista de Filosofía, 56, 171–198. https://doi.org/10.12795/themata.2017.i56.08
Ros Velasco, J. (2022). Boredom in Pandemic Times: It Won’t Make Us More Creative
(Unfortunately). Journal of Boredom Studies, 1. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6985511
Rupp, D., and Vodanovich, S. (1997). The Role of Boredom Proneness in Self-Reported
Anger and Aggression. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 12(4), 925–936.
Sandywell, B. (2016). The Dialectic of Lassitude: A Reflexive Investigation. In Boredom
Studies Reader (pp. 38–52). Routledge.
Schaefer,
C. (2003). Bored to Distraction: Cinema of Excess in End-of-TheCentury Mexico and Spain. State University of New York
Press.
Schielke, S. (2008). Boredom and Despair in Rural Egypt. Contemporary Islam,
2(3), 251–270. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11562-008-0065-8
Schneider,
G. (2012). The Thin Line between Boring and Interesting. Image &
Narrative, 3. Retrieved from http://comicsforum.org/2012/04/20/image-narrative-3-the-thin-line-between-boring-and-
interesting-by-greice-schneider/
Schopenhauer,
A. (1969). The World as Will and Representation, vol. 1. Dover
Publications.
Schubert,
D. (1977). Boredom as an Antagonist of Creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 11(4), 233–240.
Scitovsky, T. (1999). Boredom - An Overlooked Disease? Challenge, 42(5),
5–15.
Shalev,
S. (2008). A Sourcebook on Solitary Confinement. Retrieved from http://solitaryconfinement.org/uploads/sourcebook_web.pdf
Shamir, B., and Drory, A. (1982). Occupational Tedium Among Prison Officers. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 9(1),
79–99. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854882009001006
Sienkiewicz,
B. (1999). Nuda i świadomość w powieści
inteligenckiej [Boredom and Consciousness in the Intellectual
Novel]. In Nuda w kulturze [Boredom in Culture]
(pp. 142–173). Rebis.
Smith,
R. (1981). Boredom: A Review. Human Factors,
23(3), 329–340.
Śniedziewski, P. (2011). Melancholijne spojrzenie [Melancholic
Look]. Universitas.
Sontag,
S. (1967). Against Interpretation. Dell Publishing.
Spacks, P. M. (1995). Boredom: The Literary History of a State of Mind.
The Chicago University Press.
Spielberger,
C. (Ed.). (2004). Boredom. In Encyclopedia of Applied Psychology, vol. 1 (pp. 289–295). Elsevier Academic Press.
Stebbins, R. (1990). The
Laugh-Makers. Stand-up Comedy as Art, Business, and Life-Style. McGill-Queen’s University Press.
Stebbins,
R. (2003). Boredom in Free Time. Lesiure Studies Association Newsletter, 64(2), 29–31.
Steele,
R., Henderson, P., Lennon, F., and Swinden, D. (2013). Boredom Among Psychiatric
Inpatients: Does It Matter? Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 19(4),
259–267. https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363
Stets, J., and Turner, J. (Eds.). (2006).
Handbook of the Sociology of Emotions. Springer.
Sundberg,
N., Latkin, C., Farmer, R., and Saoud, J. (1991). Boredom in Young Adults: Gender
and Cultural Comparisons. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 22(2),
209–223. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022191222003
Sundström, M., Hjelm-Lindholm, S., and Radon, A.
(2019). Clicking the Boredom Away - Exploring Impulse Fashion Buying Behavior
Online. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 47, 150–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2018.11.006
Svendsen,
L. (2005). A Philosophy of Boredom. Reaktion Books.
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Sassa,
Y., Nagase, T., Nouchi, R., Kawashima, R. (2012). The
Association between Resting Functional Connectivity and Creativity. Cerebral
Cortex, 22(12), 2921–2929. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr371
Tardieu,
É. (1913). L’Ennui. Étude psychologique [Boredom. Psychologic
Study]. Libraire Félix Alcan.
Tazbir,
J. (1997). Pojęcie nudy było nieznane [The Concept of Boredom
Was Unknown]. In Polska na zakrętach
dziejów [Poland in the Twists of History] (pp.
189–198). Sic!
Thackray, R. (1981). The Stress of Boredom and Monotony: A Consideration of the
Evidence. Psychosomatic Medicine, 43(2), 165–176. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006842-198104000-00008
Thompson, W. (2006). Effects of
Shift Work and Sustained Operations: Operator Performance in Remotely PilotedAircraft (OP-REPAIR). Retrieved from https://archive.org/details/DTIC_ADA443145
Tochilnikova, E. (2020). Towards a General Theory of Boredom: A Case Study of
Anglo and Russian Society. Routledge.
Todman,
M. (2003). Boredom and Psychotic Disorders: Cognitive and Motivational Issues. Psychiatry,
66(2), 146–167. https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.66.2.146.20623
Tolor,
A. (1989). Boredom as Related to Alienation, Assertiveness, Internal-External Expectancy,
and Sleep Patterns. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45(2),
260–265. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(198903)45:2<260::AID-JCLP2270450213>3.0.CO;2-G
Tolstoy, L. (1970). The Death of
Ivan Ilyich. Signet Classics.
Toohey,
P. (1988). Some Ancient Notions of Boredom. Illinois Classical Studies, 13(1),
152–164.
Toohey,
P. (2011). Boredom: A Lively History. Yale University Press.
Tournadre, J. (2020). Between Boredom, Protest, and Community: Ethnography of
Young Activists in a South African Township. Journal of Contemporary
Ethnography, 49(3), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891241619891227
Tsai,
C.-J. (2016). Boredom at Work and Job Monotony: An Exploratory Case Study Within
the Catering Sector. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 27(2),
207–236. https://doi.org/10.1002/hrdq
Tunariu, A., and Reavey,
P. (2003). Men in Love: Living with Sexual Boredom. Sexual and Relationship
Therapy, 18(1), 63–94. https://doi.org/10.1080/1468199031000061272
Tymkiw,
A. (2017). Emotions Involved in Shopping at the Airport. The Catalyst, 4(1),
5–17. https://doi.org/10.18785/cat.0401.02
Tze, V., Daniels, L., Klassen, R., and Li, J. (2013). Canadian and Chinese University
Students’ Approaches to Coping with Academic Boredom. Learning and
Individual Differences, 23(1), 32–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.10.015
Vaneigem, R. (1994). The Revolution of
Everyday Life. Rebel Press.
Van den Berg, M., and O’Neill, B. (2017).
Introduction: Rethinking the Class Politics of Boredom. Focaal, 78, 1–8.
Van
Maanen, J. (1974). Working the Street: A Developmental View of Police Behavior. Retrieved from https://dspace.mit.edu/handle/1721.1/1873
Van Tilburg, W., and Igou, E. (2011). On
Boredom and Social Identity: A Pragmatic Meaning-Regulation Approach. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(12), 1679–1691. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211418530
Van Tilburg, W., and Igou, E. (2012). On Boredom: Lack of Challenge and Meaning
as Distinct Boredom Experiences. Motivation and Emotion, 36(2),
181–194. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9234-9
Van Tilburg, W., and Igou, E. (2017). Boredom Begs to Differ:
Differentiation from Other Negative Emotions. Emotion, 17(2),
309–322. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000233
Veblen,
T. (2007). The Theory of the Leisure Class. Oxford University Press.
Vodanovich, S., Kass, S., Andrasik, F., Gerber, W.-D., Niederberger, U., and Breaux, C. (2011). Culture and Gender
Differences in Boredom Proneness. North American Journal of Psychology, 13(2),
221–230.
Vodanovich, S., and Watt, J. (1999). The Relationship Between Time Structure
and Boredom Proneness: An Investigation Within Two Cultures. Journal of
Social Psychology, 139(2), 143. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224549909598368
Vodanovich, S., and Watt, J. (2016). Self-Report Measures of Boredom: An
Updated Review of the Literature. The Journal of Psychology, 150(2),
196–228. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2015.1074531
Volante,
W., Merz, M., Stowers, K., and Hancock, P. (2016). Sleep, Workload and Boredom:
Subject Matter Expert Insights. Proceedings of the Human Factors and
Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 60(1), 1833–1837. https://doi.org/10.1177/1541931213601418
Wallace, D. F. (2011). The Pale
King: An Unfinished Novel. Little, Brown.
Wangh,
M. (1979). Some Psychoanalytic Observations on Boredom. The International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 60(4), 515–527.
Wagner, I., and Finkielsztein, M. (2021). Strategic Boredom: The Experience
and Dynamics of Boredom in Refugee Camp. A Mediterranean Case. Journal of
Contemporary Ethnography, 50(5), 649–682. https://doi.org/10.1177/08912416211008525
Watt, J., and Vodanovich, S. (1999). Boredom Proneness and Psychosocial
Development. The Journal of Psychology, 133(3), 303–314. https://doi.org/10.1086/250095
Wegner,
L., Flisher, A., Muller, M., and Lombard, C. (2006). Leisure Boredom and Substance
Use Among High School Students in South Africa. Journal of Leisure Research,
38(2), 249–266.
Wemelsfelder, F. (1985). Animal Boredom: Is a Scientific Study of the Subjective
Experiences of Animals Possible? In Advances in Animal Welfare Science 1984
(pp. 115–154). Martinus Nijhoff Publishers.
Wilson,
T., Reinhard, D., Westgate, E., Gilbert, D., Ellerbeck, N., Hahn, C., Brown, C.
L., and Shaked, A. (2014). Just Think: The Challenges of the Disengaged Mind. Science,
345(6192), 75–77. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1250830
Zomorodi, M. (2017). Bored and Brilliant. How Time Spent Doing Nothing
Changes Everything. Palgrave Macmillan.
Zuckerman,
M. (1979). Sensation Seeking. Beyond the Optimal Level of Arousal. Taylor
& Francis Psychology Press.
[1] Boredom is a stress factor
correlated with high cortisol levels. Stress constitutes an affective reaction
for ‘unsolved’ emotion, i.e., the situation in which factors causing emotion
are unmanageable and do not recede for a longer period of time. There are
various kinds of stress, and one of these is boredom-stress, the stress
associated with prolonged inactivity, lack of engagement.